Why We Sleep: The New Science of Sleep and Dreams
Rate it:
Open Preview
Read between January 29 - April 23, 2025
54%
Flag icon
They essentially cracked the code of an individual’s dream for the very first time and, in doing so, led us to an ethically uncomfortable place.
Emre Can Okten
Created a training set.
54%
Flag icon
There may well be a time in the not-too-distant future where we can accurately “read out” and thus take ownership of a process that few people have volitional control over—the dream.fn1
55%
Flag icon
In his seminal book The Interpretation of Dreams (1899), Freud situated the dream unquestionably within the brain (that is, the mind, as there is arguably no ontological difference between the two) of an individual.
55%
Flag icon
Simply put, Freud believed that dreams came from unconscious wishes that had not been fulfilled.
55%
Flag icon
A theory that cannot be discerned true or false in this way will always be abandoned by science, and that is precisely what happened to Freud and his psychoanalytic practices.
56%
Flag icon
A meaningful, psychologically healthy life is an examined one, as Socrates so often declared.
56%
Flag icon
Nevertheless, the psychoanalytic method built on Freudian theory is nonscientific and holds no repeatable, reliable, or systematic power for decoding dreams. This, people must be made aware of.
56%
Flag icon
In actual fact, Freud knew of thi...
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
56%
Flag icon
The Interpretation of Dreams, where he states: “deeper research will one day trace the path further and discover an organic basis for the mental event.”
56%
Flag icon
Freud initially tried to construct a scientifically informed, neurobiological explanation of the mind in a work called the Project for a Scientific Psychology.
56%
Flag icon
Unfortunately, the field of neuroscience was still in its infancy at the time.
56%
Flag icon
Since autobiographical memory regions of the brain, including the hippocampus, are so active during REM sleep, we should expect dreaming to contain elements of the individual’s recent experience and perhaps give clues as to the meaning, if any, of dreams: something that Freud elegantly described as “day residue.”
56%
Flag icon
Dreams are not, therefore, a wholesale replay of our waking lives. We do not simply rewind the video of the day’s recorded experience and relive it at night, projected on the big screen of our cortex.
56%
Flag icon
If there is a red-thread narrative that runs from our waking lives into our dreaming lives, it is that of emotional concerns.
56%
Flag icon
But do dreams themselves, above and beyond REM sleep, actually do anything for us? As a matter of scientific fact, yes, they do.
56%
Flag icon
Chapter 10 Dreaming as Overnight Therapy
57%
Flag icon
Using this framework, we have found two core benefits of REM sleep. Both functional benefits require not just that you have REM sleep, but that you dream, and dream about specific things. REM sleep is necessary, but REM sleep alone is not sufficient. Dreams are not the heat of the lightbulb—they are no by-product.
57%
Flag icon
REM-sleep dreaming takes the painful sting out of difficult, even traumatic, emotional episodes you have experienced during the day, offering emotional resolution when you awake the next morning.
57%
Flag icon
REM sleep is the only time during the twenty-four-hour period when your brain is completely devoid of this anxiety-triggering molecule.
57%
Flag icon
Is the REM-sleep dreaming state a perfectly designed nocturnal soothing balm—one that removes the emotional sharp edges of our daily lives? It seemed so from everything neurobiology and neurophysiology was telling us (me).
57%
Flag icon
If true, it would suggest that the dream state supports a form of introspective life review, to therapeutic ends.
57%
Flag icon
You have not forgotten the memory, but you have cast off the emotional charge, or at least a significant amount of it.
57%
Flag icon
The theory argued that we have REM-sleep dreaming to thank for this palliative dissolving of emotion from experience.
57%
Flag icon
Indeed, I argued that if REM sleep did not perform this operation, we’d all be left with a state of chronic anxiety in our autobiographical memory networks; every time we recalled something salient, not only would we recall the details of the memory, but we would relive the same stressful emotional charge all over again.
57%
Flag icon
Those who slept in between the two sessions reported a significant decrease in how emotional they were feeling in response to seeing those images again.
57%
Flag icon
contrast, those who remained awake across the day without the chance to sleep and digest those experiences showed no such dissolving of emotional reactivity over time.
57%
Flag icon
Is there something about the type or quality of sleep that an individual experiences that predicts how successful sleep is at accomplishing next-day emotional resolution?
58%
Flag icon
As the theory predicted, it was the dreaming state of REM sleep—and specific patterns of electrical activity that reflected the drop in stress-related brain chemistry during the dream state—that determined the success of overnight therapy from one individual to the next.
58%
Flag icon
Cartwright had shown that it was not enough to have REM sleep, or even generic dreaming, when it comes to resolving our emotional past. Her patients required REM sleep with dreaming, but dreaming of a very specific kind: that which expressly involved dreaming about the emotional themes and sentiments of the waking trauma.
58%
Flag icon
If the brain cannot divorce the emotion from memory across the first night following a trauma experience, the theory suggests that a repeat attempt of emotional memory stripping will occur on the second night, as the strength of the “emotional tag” associated with the memory remains too high. If the process fails a second time, the same attempt will continue to repeat the next night, and the next night, like a broken record.
59%
Flag icon
Prazosin was gradually lowering the harmful high tide of noradrenaline within the brain, giving these patients healthier REM-sleep quality. With healthy REM sleep came a reduction in the patients’ clinical symptoms and, most critically, a decrease in the frequency of their repetitive nightmares.
59%
Flag icon
prazosin has now been used by the VA for the treatment of repetitive trauma nightmares, and has since received approval by the US Food and Drug Administration for the same benefit.
59%
Flag icon
There are regions of your brain whose job it is to read and decode the value and meaning of emotional signals, especially faces. And it is that very same essential set of brain regions, or network, that REM sleep recalibrates at night.
59%
Flag icon
Deprive an individual of their REM-sleep dreaming state, and the emotional tuning curve of the brain loses its razor-sharp precision.
59%
Flag icon
Like viewing an image through frosted glass, or looking at an out-of-focus picture, a dream-starved brain cannot accurately decode facial expressions, which become distorted. You begin to mistake friends for foes.
59%
Flag icon
the better the quality of REM sleep from one individual to the next across that rested night, the more precise the tuning within the emotional decoding networks of the brain the next day.
59%
Flag icon
Through this platinum-grade nocturnal service, better REM-sleep quality at night provided superior comprehension of the social world the next day.
59%
Flag icon
By removing REM sleep, we had, quite literally, removed participants’ levelheaded ability to read the social world around them.
60%
Flag icon
But come the early teenage years and the inflection point of parental independence wherein an adolescent must navigate the socioemotional world for himself, now we see the young brain feasting on this emotional recalibration benefit of REM sleep.
60%
Flag icon
Chapter 11 Dream Creativity and Dream Control
60%
Flag icon
REM sleep and the act of dreaming have another distinct benefit: intelligent information processing that inspires creativity and promotes problem solving.
60%
Flag icon
We awake with a revised “Mind Wide Web” that is capable of divining solutions to previously impenetrable problems. In this way, REM-sleep dreaming is informational alchemy.
60%
Flag icon
I saw in a dream a table where all the elements fell into place as required. Awakening, I immediately wrote it down on a piece of paper.
61%
Flag icon
And what is so special about the neurophysiology of REM sleep that would explain these creative benefits, and the dreaming obligate to them?
61%
Flag icon
Upon enforced awakening, the brain’s neurophysiology starts out far more sleep-like than wake-like and, with each passing minute, the concentration of the prior sleep stage from which an individual has been woken will gradually fade from the brain as true wakefulness rises to the surface.
61%
Flag icon
The lingering vapors of REM sleep were providing a more fluid, divergent, “open-minded” state of information processing.
61%
Flag icon
Using a standard computer test, Stickgold measured how these associative networks of information operated following NREM-sleep and REM-sleep awakenings, and during standard performance during the waking day.
62%
Flag icon
The REM-sleep dreaming brain was utterly uninterested in bland, commonsense links—the one-step-to-the-next associations. Instead, the REM-sleep brain was shortcutting the obvious links and favoring very distantly related concepts.
62%
Flag icon
As we enter REM sleep and dreaming takes hold, an inspired form of memory mixology begins to occur. No longer are we constrained to see the most typical and plainly obvious connections between memory units. On the contrary, the brain becomes actively biased toward seeking out the most distant, nonobvious links between sets of information.
62%
Flag icon
the content of people’s dreams, above and beyond simply having REM sleep, should determine the success of those hyper-associative problem-solving benefits.