Why We Sleep: The New Science of Sleep and Dreams
Rate it:
Open Preview
Read between January 29 - April 23, 2025
31%
Flag icon
You will recall from chapter 3 that we obtain most of our deep NREM sleep early in the night, and much of our REM sleep (and lighter NREM sleep) late in the night.
32%
Flag icon
It was early-night sleep, rich in deep NREM, that won out in terms of providing superior memory retention savings relative to late-night, REM-rich sleep.
32%
Flag icon
the more deep NREM sleep, the more information an individual remembered the next day.
32%
Flag icon
Before having slept, participants were fetching memories from the short-term storage site of the hippocampus—that temporary warehouse,
32%
Flag icon
After the full night of sleep, participants were now retrieving that same information from the neocortex,
32%
Flag icon
region that serves as the long-term storage site for fact-based memories,
32%
Flag icon
By transferring memories of yesterday from the short-term repository of the hippocampus to the long-term home within the cortex, you awake with both yesterday’s experiences safely filed away and having regained your short-term storage capacity for new learning throughout that following day.
32%
Flag icon
The cycle repeats each day and night, clearing out the cache of short-term memory for the new imprinting of facts, while accumulating an ever-updated catalog of past memories.
32%
Flag icon
Sleep is constantly modifying the information architecture of...
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
32%
Flag icon
At every stage of human life, the relationship between NREM sleep and memory solidification is therefore observed.
32%
Flag icon
In other words, following a night of sleep you regain access to memories that you could not retrieve before sleep.
33%
Flag icon
By boosting the electrical quality of deep-sleep brainwave activity, the researchers almost doubled the number of facts that individuals were able to recall the following day, relative to those participants who received no stimulation.
33%
Flag icon
One technology involves quiet auditory tones being played over speakers next to the sleeper.
33%
Flag icon
the auditory stimulation increased the power of the slow brainwaves and returned an impressive 40 percent memory enhancement the next morning.
33%
Flag icon
Sleep stimulation methods are promising, but they do have a potential limitation: the memory benefit they provide is indiscriminate. That is, all things learned before sleep are generally enhanced the next day.
33%
Flag icon
What if a similar opportunity was possible with sleep and memory? Before going to bed, you would review the learning experiences of the day, selecting only those memories from the menu list that you would like improved. You place your order, then go to sleep, knowing that your order will be served to you overnight.
33%
Flag icon
It all sounds like the stuff of science fiction, but it is now science fact: the method is called targeted memory reactivation.
33%
Flag icon
Moreover, forgetting is not just beneficial to delete stored information we no longer need. It also lowers the brain resources required for retrieving those memories we want to retain, similar to the ease of finding important documents on a neatly organized, clutter-free desk.
34%
Flag icon
Instead, sleep is able to offer a far more discerning hand in memory improvement: one that preferentially picks and chooses what information is, and is not, ultimately strengthened. Sleep accomplishes this by using meaningful tags that have been hung onto those memories during initial learning, or potentially identified during sleep itself.
34%
Flag icon
Rather, it was NREM sleep, and especially the very quickest of the sleep spindles that helped bend apart the curves of remembering and forgetting.
34%
Flag icon
The recursive cycle of activity between these two areas (memory and intentionality), which happens ten to fifteen times per second during the spindles, may help explain NREM sleep’s discerning memory influence.
34%
Flag icon
We are now exploring ways of harnessing this remarkably intelligent service of selective remembering and forgetting with painful or problematic memories.
34%
Flag icon
You can only learn how to ride a bike by doing rather than reading. Which is to say by practicing. The same is true for all motor skills, whether you are learning a musical instrument, an athletic sport, a surgical procedure, or how to fly a plane.
34%
Flag icon
The term “muscle memory” is a misnomer. Muscles themselves have no such memory: a muscle that is not connected to a brain cannot perform any skilled actions, nor does a muscle store skilled routines. Muscle memory is, in fact, brain memory.
34%
Flag icon
Training and strengthening muscles can help you better execute a skilled memory routine. But the routine itself—the memory program—resides f...
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
35%
Flag icon
“As a pianist,” he said, “I have an experience that seems far too frequent to be chance. I will be practicing a particular piece, even late into the evening, and I cannot seem to master it. Often, I make the same mistake at the same place in a particular movement. I go to bed frustrated. But when I wake up the next morning and sit back down at the piano, I can just play, perfectly.”
35%
Flag icon
In other words, your brain will continue to improve skill memories in the absence of any further practice. It is really quite magical. Yet, that delayed, “offline” learning occurs exclusively across a period of sleep, and not across equivalent time periods spent awake, regardless of whether the time awake or time asleep comes first.
35%
Flag icon
Even after a long period of initial training, participants would consistently struggle with particular transitions within the sequence. These problem points stuck out like a sore thumb when I looked at the speed of the keystrokes.
35%
Flag icon
Rather than a transfer from short- to long-term memory required for saving facts, the motor memories had been shifted over to brain circuits that operate below the level of consciousness. As a result, those skill actions were now instinctual habits.
35%
Flag icon
The increases in speed and accuracy, underpinned by efficient automaticity, were directly related to the amount of stage 2 NREM, especially in the last two hours of an eight-hour night of sleep (e.g., from five to seven a.m., should you have fallen asleep at eleven p.m.).
36%
Flag icon
Perhaps more relevant to the modern world is the time-of-night effect we discovered. Those last two hours of sleep are precisely the window that many of us feel it is okay to cut short to get a jump start on the day. As a result, we miss out on this feast of late-morning sleep spindles.
36%
Flag icon
Obtain anything less than eight hours of sleep a night, and especially less than six hours a night, and the following happens: time to physical exhaustion drops by 10 to 30 percent, and aerobic output is significantly reduced.
36%
Flag icon
In the context of injury, there is no better risk-mitigating insurance policy for these investments than sleep. Described in a research study of competitive young athletes in 2014,fn11 you can see that a chronic lack of sleep across the season predicted a markedly higher risk of injury (figure 10
36%
Flag icon
Post-performance sleep accelerates physical recovery from common inflammation, stimulates muscle repair, and helps restock cellular energy in the form of glucose and glycogen.
37%
Flag icon
In ways your waking brain would never attempt, the sleeping brain fuses together disparate sets of knowledge that foster impressive problem-solving abilities.
37%
Flag icon
For now, I will simply tell you that such informational alchemy conjured by REM-sleep dreaming has led to some of the greatest feats of transformative thinking in the history of the human race.
37%
Flag icon
Chapter 7 Too Extreme for the Guinness Book of World Records
37%
Flag icon
We are, as you will see, socially, organizationally, economically, physically, behaviorally, nutritionally, linguistically, cognitively, and emotionally dependent upon sleep.
37%
Flag icon
One brain function that buckles under even the smallest dose of sleep deprivation is concentration.
37%
Flag icon
The second, more common cause is a momentary lapse in concentration, called a microsleep.
37%
Flag icon
They are usually suffered by individuals who are chronically sleep restricted, defined as getting less than seven hours of sleep a night on a routine basis.
38%
Flag icon
Ten days of six hours of sleep a night was all it took to become as impaired in performance as going without sleep for twenty-four hours straight.
38%
Flag icon
When participants were asked about their subjective sense of how impaired they were, they consistently underestimated their degree of performance disability.
38%
Flag icon
With chronic sleep restriction over months or years, an individual will actually acclimate to their impaired performance, lower alertness, and reduced energy levels.
38%
Flag icon
That low-level exhaustion becomes their accepted norm, or baseline. Individuals fail to recognize how their perennial state of sleep deficiency has come to compromise their mental aptitude and physical vitality, including the slow accumulation of ill health.
38%
Flag icon
Nor did any group recover all the sleep hours they had lost in the days prior. As we have already learned, the brain is incapable of that.
39%
Flag icon
Note how the relationship between decreasing hours of sleep and increasing mortality risk of an accident is not linear, but instead exponentially mushrooms.
39%
Flag icon
The heady cocktail of sleep loss and alcohol was not additive, but instead multiplicative
39%
Flag icon
The recycle rate of a human being is around sixteen hours. After sixteen hours of being awake, the brain begins to fail. Humans need more than seven hours of sleep each night to maintain cognitive performance.
39%
Flag icon
Three full nights of recovery sleep (i.e., more nights than a weekend) are insufficient to restore performance back to normal levels after a week of short sleeping.
1 5 10