The Paradox of Choice: Why More Is Less
Rate it:
Open Preview
Kindle Notes & Highlights
Read between January 23 - January 28, 2025
2%
Flag icon
As a culture, we are enamored of freedom, self-determination, and variety, and we are reluctant to give up any of our options. But clinging tenaciously to all the choices available to us contributes to bad decisions, to anxiety, stress, and dissatisfaction—even to clinical depression.
3%
Flag icon
Instead, I believe that we make the most of our freedoms by learning to make good choices about the things that matter, while at the same time unburdening ourselves from too much concern about the things that don’t.
6%
Flag icon
myopic.
8%
Flag icon
EXTRANEOUS
9%
Flag icon
According to a survey conducted by Yankelovich Partners, a majority of people want more control over the details of their lives, but a majority of people also want to simplify their lives. There you have it—the paradox of our times.
10%
Flag icon
perfunctory
12%
Flag icon
The doctors are now merely instruments for the execution of our decisions.
16%
Flag icon
Nobel Prize–winning psychologist Daniel Kahneman and his colleagues have shown that what we remember about the pleasurable quality of our past experiences is almost entirely determined by two things: how the experiences felt when they were at their peak (best or worst), and how they felt when they ended.
17%
Flag icon
In the same way, we evaluate positive experiences on the basis of how good they feel at their best, and how good they feel at the end. Thus, you might, in retrospect, remember a one-week vacation that had some great moments and finished with a bang as more pleasurable than a three-week vacation that also had some great moments, but finished only with a whimper.
17%
Flag icon
So it seems that neither our predictions about how we will feel after an experience nor our memories of how we did feel during the experience are very accurate reflections of how we actually do feel while the experience is occurring. And yet it is memories of the past and expectations for the future that govern our choices.
19%
Flag icon
The availability heuristic says that we assume that the more available some piece of information is to memory, the more frequently we must have encountered it in the past. This heuristic is partly true. In general, the frequency of experience does affect its availability to memory. But frequency of experience is not the only thing that affects availability to memory. Salience or vividness matters as well.
20%
Flag icon
propensity
20%
Flag icon
CNN or USA Today tell everyone in the country, and now even the world, the same story, which makes it less likely that an individual’s biased understanding of the evidence will be corrected by his friends and neighbors. Those friends and neighbors will have the same biased understanding, derived from the same source. When you hear the same story everywhere you look and listen, you assume it must be true. And the more people believe it’s true, the more likely they are to repeat it, and thus the more likely you are to hear it. This is how inaccurate information can create a bandwagon effect, ...more
23%
Flag icon
“law of diminishing marginal utility.” As the rich get richer, each additional unit of wealth satisfies them less.
23%
Flag icon
“decreasing marginal disutility of losses.”
23%
Flag icon
Some studies have estimated that losses have more than twice the psychological impact as equivalent gains. The fact is, we all hate to lose, which Kahneman and Tversky refer to as loss aversion.
24%
Flag icon
discounts and surcharges are just two ways of saying the same thing.
24%
Flag icon
endowment effect.
24%
Flag icon
prospect theory tells us, because losses are more bad than gains are good,
24%
Flag icon
sunk costs that
25%
Flag icon
Thus the growth of options and opportunities for choice has three, related, unfortunate effects.   It means that decisions require more effort. It makes mistakes more likely. It makes the psychological consequences of mistakes more severe.
28%
Flag icon
Our expectation was confirmed: people with high maximization scores experienced less satisfaction with life, were less happy, were less optimistic, and were more depressed than people with low maximization scores. In fact, people with extreme maximization scores—scores of 65 or more out of 91—had depression scores that placed them in the borderline clinical depression range.
30%
Flag icon
While maximizers and perfectionists both have very high standards, I think that perfectionists have very high standards that they don’t expect to meet, whereas maximizers have very high standards that they do expect to meet.
34%
Flag icon
But if money doesn’t do it for people, what does? What seems to be the most important factor in providing happiness is close social relations. People who are married, who have good friends, and who are close to their families are happier than those who are not. People who participate in religious communities are happier than those who do not.
35%
Flag icon
Both books point out how the growth of material affluence has not brought with it an increase in subjective well-being. But they go further. Both books argue that we are actually experiencing a fairly significant decrease in well-being.
35%
Flag icon
There are too many life choices…without concern for the resulting overload…and the lack of constraint by custom…that is, demands to discover or create an identity rather than to accept a given identity.
35%
Flag icon
Lane writes that we are paying for increased affluence and increased freedom with a substantial decrease in the quality and quantity of social relations.
36%
Flag icon
Time spent dealing with choice is time taken away from being a good friend, a good spouse, a good parent, and a good congregant.
37%
Flag icon
So by using rules, presumptions, standards, and routines to constrain ourselves and limit the decisions we face, we can make life more manageable, which gives us more time to devote ourselves to other people and to the decisions that we can’t or don’t want to avoid.
37%
Flag icon
But powerful evidence has recently appeared that “wanting” and “liking” are served by fundamentally different brain systems—systems that often do, but certainly need not, work together.
43%
Flag icon
There is a great deal of evidence that negative emotional states of mind narrow our focus. Instead of examining all aspects of a decision, we home in on only one or two, perhaps ignoring aspects of the decision that are very important.
43%
Flag icon
Negative emotion also distracts us, inducing us to focus on the emotion rather than on the decision itself.
43%
Flag icon
More recent evidence has shown that positive emotion has the opposite effect—when we are in a good mood, we think better.
47%
Flag icon
After millions of years of survival based on simple distinctions, it may simply be that we are biologically unprepared for the number of choices we face in the modern world.
49%
Flag icon
omission bias, a bias to downplay omissions (failures to act) when we evaluate the consequences of our decisions.
50%
Flag icon
counterfactual thinking,
54%
Flag icon
ameliorate
54%
Flag icon
adaptation.
56%
Flag icon
hedonic adaptations
56%
Flag icon
But as people adapt—as the novelty wears off—pleasure comes to be replaced by comfort. It’s a thrill to drive your new car for the first few weeks; after that, it’s just comfortable. It certainly beats the old car, but it isn’t much of a kick. Comfort is nice enough, but people want pleasure. And comfort isn’t pleasure.
58%
Flag icon
“amortized”
59%
Flag icon
But individuals who regularly experience and express gratitude are physically healthier, more optimistic about the future, and feel better about their lives than those who do not. Individuals who experience gratitude are more alert, enthusiastic, and energetic than those who do not, and they are more likely to achieve personal goals.
61%
Flag icon
The proliferation of options seems to lead, inexorably, to the raising of expectations.
64%
Flag icon
Happy people have the ability to distract themselves and move on, whereas unhappy people get stuck ruminating and make themselves more and more miserable.
66%
Flag icon
It seems that as American society grows wealthier and Americans become freer to pursue and do whatever they want, Americans get less and less happy.
66%
Flag icon
and severely depressed individuals miss five times as much.
66%
Flag icon
Seligman speculated that at least some instances of clinical depression were the result of individuals’ having experienced one significant loss of control over their lives and then coming to believe that they were helpless, that they could expect this helplessness to persist into the future and to be present across a wide range of different circumstances.
71%
Flag icon
Having too many choices produces psychological distress, especially when combined with regret, concern about status, adaptation, social comparison, and perhaps most important, the desire to have the best of everything—to maximize.
73%
Flag icon
diminution