How We Learn: The Surprising Truth About When, Where, and Why It Happens
Rate it:
Open Preview
1%
Flag icon
I was driven more by a fear of falling than by anything like curiosity or wonder.
Christian Jespersen
Key argument
2%
Flag icon
known. I’m not saying that I majored in gin and tonics; I never let go of my studies—just allowed them to become part of my life, rather than its central purpose. And somewhere in that tangle of good living and bad, I became a student.
Christian Jespersen
Fantastisk citat til online kursus
2%
Flag icon
For years afterward, I thought about college like I suspect many people do: I’d performed pretty well despite my scattered existence, my bad habits. I never stopped to ask whether those habits were, in fact, bad.
2%
Flag icon
Distractions can aid learning. Napping does, too. Quitting before a project is done: not all bad, as an almost done project lingers in memory far longer than one that is completed. Taking a test on a subject before you know anything about it improves subsequent learning.
4%
Flag icon
In short, it is not that there is a right way and wrong way to learn. It’s that there are different strategies, each uniquely suited to capturing a particular type of information. A good hunter tailors the trap to the prey.
4%
Flag icon
But shooting for a goal so vague puts a person at risk of worshiping an ideal—and missing the target.
9%
Flag icon
And it’s this instant access that creates what to me is the brain’s grandest illusion: that memories are “filed away” like video scenes that can be opened with a neural click, and snapped closed again. The truth is stranger—and far more useful.
11%
Flag icon
As scientists put it, using our memories changes our memories.
12%
Flag icon
memory. And photographic memories, as far as scientists can tell, don’t exist, at least not in the way that we imagine.
Christian Jespersen
Til senere brug
13%
Flag icon
nothing.” The study of forgetting has, in the past few decades, forced a fundamental reconsideration of how learning works. In a way, it has also altered what the words “remember” and “forget” mean. “The relationship between learning and forgetting is not so simple and in certain important respects is quite the opposite of what people assume,” Robert Bjork, a psychologist at the University of California, Los Angeles, told
13%
Flag icon
I think of this as the muscle-building property of forgetting: Some “breakdown” must occur for us to strengthen learning when we revisit the material. Without a little forgetting, you get no benefit from further study. It is what allows learning to build, like an exercised muscle.
13%
Flag icon
The point is not that memory is nothing more than a pile of loose facts and a catalog of tall tales. It’s that retrieving any memory alters its accessibility, and often its content. There is an emerging theory that accounts for these and related ideas. It’s called the New Theory of Disuse, to distinguish it from an older, outdated principle stating, simply, that memories evaporate entirely from the brain over time if they’re not used. The new theory is far more than an updating, though. It’s an overhaul, recasting forgetting as the best friend of learning, rather than its rival.
13%
Flag icon
A better name for it, then, might be the Forget to Learn theory. That phrase captures its literal implications and its general spirit, its reassuring voice. One implication, for instance, is that forgetting a huge chunk of what we’ve just learned, especially when it’s a brand-new topic, is not necessarily evidence of laziness, attention deficits, or a faulty character. On the contrary, it is a sign that the brain is working as it should.
15%
Flag icon
In 1914, the influential American education researcher Edward Thorndike turned Ebbinghaus’s curve into a “law” of learning. He called it the Law of Disuse, which asserted that learned information, without continued use, decays from memory entirely—i.e., use it or lose it.
16%
Flag icon
Memory improved in the first few days without any further study, and only began to taper off after day four or so, on average.
16%
Flag icon
seems to have caused mostly confusion. Few scientists appreciated what he’d done, and even today he is little more than a footnote in psychology, a far more obscure figure than Ebbinghaus. Still, Ballard knew what he had. “We not only tend to forget what we have once remembered,” he wrote, “but we also tend to remember what we have once forgotten.” Memory does not have just one tendency over time, toward decay. It has two. The other—“reminiscence,” Ballard called it—is a kind of growth, a bubbling up of facts or words that we don’t recall having learned in the first place. Both tendencies ...more
17%
Flag icon
The American psychologist B. F. Skinner showed how rewards and punishments could alter behavior, and accelerate learning in many circumstances. Skinner tested various reward schedules against one another and got striking results: An automatic reward for a correct answer leads to little learning; occasional, periodic rewards are much more effective. Skinner’s work, which was enormously influential among educators, focused on improving teaching, rather than on the peculiarities of memory.
17%
Flag icon
You can’t practice what you don’t remember.
Christian Jespersen
Gc
17%
Flag icon
Why does recall of pictures improve while recall of word lists does not?
18%
Flag icon
The first principle theory is this: Any memory has two strengths, a storage strength and a retrieval strength. Storage strength is just that, a measure of how well learned something is. It builds up steadily with studying, and more sharply with use. The multiplication table is a good example. It’s drilled into our heads in grade school, and we use it continually throughout life, in a wide variety of situations, from balancing the bank account to calculating tips to helping our fourth grader with homework. Its storage strength is enormous.
18%
Flag icon
According to the Bjorks’ theory, storage strength can increase but it never decreases.
18%
Flag icon
The brain holds on to only what’s relevant, useful, or interesting—or may be so in the future. It does mean that everything we have deliberately committed to memory—the multiplication table, a childhood phone number, the combination to our first locker—is all there, and for good.
18%
Flag icon
accessible. Its retrieval strength is low, or near zero. Retrieval strength, on the other hand, is a measure of how easily a nugget of information comes to mind. It, too, increases with studying, and with use. Without reinforcement, however, retrieval strength drops off quickly, and its capacity is relatively small (compared to storage).
Christian Jespersen
Vigtig note
18%
Flag icon
Compared to storage, retrieval strength is fickle. It can build quickly but also weaken quickly.
19%
Flag icon
Retrieval is a matter of how quickly a person’s name comes to mind. Storage, by contrast, is a matter of how familiar the person is.
19%
Flag icon
The harder we have to work to retrieve a memory, the greater the subsequent spike in retrieval and storage strength
19%
Flag icon
“Compared to some kind of system in which out-of-date memories were to be overwritten or erased,” Bjork writes, “having such memories become inaccessible but remain in storage has important advantages. Because those memories are inaccessible, they don’t interfere with current information and procedures. But because they remain in memory they can—at least under certain circumstances—be relearned.”
19%
Flag icon
Thus, forgetting is critical to the learning of new skills and to the preservation and reacquisition of old ones.
20%
Flag icon
Yes, the Hesperus will eventually sink if the brain stops thinking about it, and its retrieval strength will inch toward zero. But a third test, and a fourth, would anchor the poem in memory more richly still, as the brain—now being called on to use the poem regularly—would continue its search for patterns within the poem, perhaps pulling up another half line or two with each exam. Will it all come back, with enough testing, even if only half was remembered the first time? Not likely. You get something back, not everything.
20%
Flag icon
Using memory changes memory—and for the better. Forgetting enables and deepens learning, by filtering out
20%
Flag icon
distracting information and by allowing some breakdown that, after reuse, drives retrieval and storage strength higher than they were originally.
20%
Flag icon
Don’t forget your brain vitamins. In college, that’s what passed for exam-taking advice,
20%
Flag icon
Yet we felt like we had a lucky charm, a way to put our head “in the same place” during test-taking as during studying. Essential it was, too, especially during finals week, with two and sometimes three tests falling on the same day. That kind of pressure drives people deep into their worst habits, whether chocolate and cigarettes, brain vitamins and nail-biting, cases of diet cola, or much stronger stuff. When hunkered down in this psychological survival mode, it can be a profound comfort to believe that a favorite “study aid” also improves exam performance. And so we did. “Brain chemistry,” ...more
21%
Flag icon
of theorizing as pure rationalization, the undergraduate mind at its self-justifying finest. We had so many crackpot theories then, about dating and getting rich and studying, that I’d discarded the whole list. Still, millions of students have developed some version of the brain chemistry idea, and I think its enduring attraction is rooted in something deeper than wishful thinking. The theory fits in nicely with what we’ve been told about good study habits from Day 1—be consistent. Consistency has been a hallmark of education manuals since the 1900s, and the principle is built into our every ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
21%
Flag icon
when in the same state of mind as when we studied—and, yes, that includes mild states of intoxication from alcohol or pot, as well as arousal from stimulants.
21%
Flag icon
The psychologists, D. R. Godden and A. D. Baddeley, wanted to test a hypothesis that many learning theorists favored: that people remember more of what they studied when they return to that same study environment.
21%
Flag icon
to what the Estonian psychologist Endel Tulving called “semantic memories.”
22%
Flag icon
and the two psychologists concluded that, “recall is better if the environment of the original learning is reinstated.”
Christian Jespersen
Du husker bedre hvad du lærer, hvis miljøet du lærer det I, et magen til miljøet du bliver testet i. Det kan bruges til Fjernstudie.
22%
Flag icon
recall. In one investigation, for example, people who studied a list of nonsense syllables on blue-gray cards remembered 20 percent more of them on a later test when the test cards were also blue-gray (as opposed to, say, red).
22%
Flag icon
The answer was no.
23%
Flag icon
strategy. The first is that our assumptions about learning are suspect, if not wrong. Having something going on in the study environment, like music, is better than nothing (so much for sanctity of the quiet study room). The second point is that the experience of studying has more dimensions than we notice, some of which can have an impact on retention. The contextual cues scientists describe—music, light, background colors—are annoyingly ephemeral, it’s true. They’re subconscious, usually untraceable. Nonetheless, it is possible to recognize them at work in our own lives.
Christian Jespersen
Når jeg skal lære noget (vigtigt) læg mærke til omgivelserne og brug dem som støtte til at huske det jeg lærer bedre. Eksempelvis det her stykke viden. Det lærte jeg i et tog på vej til Annecy, da jeg rejste med Helen.
24%
Flag icon
Moods color everything we do, and when they’re extreme they can determine what we remember.
25%
Flag icon
from all this work on external and internal cues is of a shifty-eyed dinner companion. It is tracking the main conversation (the homework assignment, the music notation, the hard facts) and occasionally becoming engaged in it. At the same time, it’s also periodically having a quick look around, taking in the room, sketching in sights and sounds and smells, as well as noting its internal reactions, its feelings and sensations. These features—the background music, a flickering candle, a pang of hunger—help our companion recall points made during the conversation later on, especially when the ...more
Christian Jespersen
Omgivelserne og de input du får hjælper med at huske hvor du lærte noget. Men, det betyder også at du skal have det samme musik, når du er til eksamen. Det er OK at lytte til musik til opgaver som du ikke skal forsvare (speciale booster)
28%
Flag icon
too? It is. Yet the larger message of context research is that, in the end, it doesn’t much matter which aspects of the environment you vary, so long as you vary what you can. The philosopher John Locke once described the case of a man who had learned to dance by practicing according to a strict ritual, always in the same room, which contained an old trunk. Unfortunately, wrote Locke, “the idea of this remarkable piece of household stuff had so mixed itself with the turns and steps of all his dances, that though in that chamber he could dance excellently well, yet it was only when that trunk ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
Christian Jespersen
Undgaa at have rutiner naar du studerer. Naar d aendrer miljoet husker du det bere foordi du forbinder det laert me hvor dublaerste/laeste det. Brug tid paa skabe et mentalt billede af hvor ddu bsidder og lavet lektier og skal til at lare indenn du begynder. Fjernstudie
29%
Flag icon
The technique is called distributed learning or, more commonly, the spacing effect. People learn at least as much, and retain it much longer, when they distribute—or “space”—their study time than when they concentrate it. Mom’s right, it is better to do a little today and a little tomorrow rather than everything at once. Not just better, a lot better. Distributed learning, in certain situations, can double the amount we remember later on.
Christian Jespersen
Fjernstudie - hvorfor det er en god ide
29%
Flag icon
This isn’t to say that cramming is useless. The all-nighter is timetested, with a long track record of improving exam scores the next day. In terms of reliability, though, this nocturnal sprint is a little like overstuffing a cheap suitcase: the contents hold for a while, then everything falls out. Researchers who study learning say the result from habitual cramming can be dramatic from one semester to the next. The students who do it “arrive for the second term, and they don’t remember anything from the first term,” Henry Roediger III, a psychologist at Washington University in St. Louis, ...more
Christian Jespersen
Skriv blog indlaeg: Lad vaere med at cramme
29%
Flag icon
The spacing effect is especially useful for memorizing new material. Try it yourself with two lists of, say, fifteen phone numbers or Russian vocabulary words. Study one list for ten minutes today and ten minutes tomorrow, and the other for twenty minutes tomorrow. Wait a week and test yourself to see how many of the total from both lists you can remember. Now go back to the two lists: The difference in what you recalled from each should be significant, and there’s no obvious explanation for it. I
Christian Jespersen
Konkret metode til at huske bedre
31%
Flag icon
“Four Bahrick Study,”
Christian Jespersen
Teori til sproglaering
32%
Flag icon
He continued to refine the ideal intervals for keeping his English sharp, and programmed a computer to track his progress. “These optimum intervals are calculated on the basis of two contradictory criteria,” he wrote at the time. “Intervals should be as long as possible to obtain the minimum frequency of repetitions, and to make the best use of the so-called spacing effect … Intervals should be short enough to ensure that the knowledge is still remembered.”
Christian Jespersen
Spaced repeetition forklaret
32%
Flag icon
intervals. To build and retain foreign vocabulary, scientific definitions, or other factual information, it’s best to review the material one or two days after initial study; then a week later; then about a month later. After that, the intervals are longer.
Christian Jespersen
Hvordn laerer vi bedst sprog og opbygget et ordforraad
« Prev 1 3 4