How We Learn: The Surprising Truth About When, Where, and Why It Happens
Rate it:
Open Preview
Kindle Notes & Highlights
55%
Flag icon
percolation.
56%
Flag icon
percolation is for building something that was not there before, whether it’s a term paper, a robot, an orchestral piece, or some other labyrinthine project.
56%
Flag icon
Perhaps unfinished jobs or goals linger in memory longer than finished ones.
57%
Flag icon
Interestingly, being interrupted at the “worst” time seemed to extend memory the longest.
57%
Flag icon
A goal is anything we want to possess or achieve and haven’t yet,
57%
Flag icon
the act of starting work on an assignment often gives that job the psychological weight of a goal, even if it’s meaningless.
57%
Flag icon
interrupting yourself when absorbed in an assignment extends its life in memory and—according to her experiments—pushes it to the top of your mental to-do list.
58%
Flag icon
The first element of percolation, then, is that supposed enemy of learning—interruption.
58%
Flag icon
Having a goal foremost in mind (in this case, a drink), tunes our perceptions to fulfilling it.
59%
Flag icon
“Once a goal becomes activated, it trumps all others and begins to drive our perceptions, our thoughts, our attitudes,”
59%
Flag icon
So the question is: How, then, do we most effectively activate that goal? By interrupting work on it at an important and difficult moment—propelling the assignment, via the Zeigarnik effect, to the top of our mind.
60%
Flag icon
The problem is that, when we’re embarking on a research project—especially when we’re younger—we don’t necessarily know how to identify those intellectual landmarks. Often, we don’t even know they exist.
61%
Flag icon
Percolation is a matter of vigilance, of finding ways to tune the mind so that it collects a mix of external perceptions and internal thoughts that are relevant to the project at hand. We can’t know in advance what those perceptions and thoughts will look like—and we don’t have to.
61%
Flag icon
If more fully formed ideas (as opposed to perceptions) seem to arrive “out of the blue,” it only means that that mixing happened outside of direct conscious awareness.
61%
Flag icon
What does this mean for a learning strategy? It suggests that we should start work on large projects as soon as possible and stop when we get stuck, with the confidence that
61%
Flag icon
Quitting before I’m ahead doesn’t put the project to sleep; it keeps it awake. That’s Phase 1, and it initiates Phase 2, the period of gathering string, of casual data collecting. Phase 3 is listening to what I think about all those incoming bits and pieces. Percolation depends on all three elements, and in that order.
63%
Flag icon
Varied practice is more effective than the focused kind, because it forces us to internalize general rules of motor adjustment that apply to any hittable target.
64%
Flag icon
Transfer is what learning is all about, really. It’s the ability to extract the essence of a skill or a formula or word problem and apply it in another context, to another problem that may not look the same, at least superficially. If you’ve truly mastered a skill, you “carry it with you,” so to speak.
65%
Flag icon
But repetition creates a powerful illusion. Skills improve quickly and then plateau.
65%
Flag icon
varied practice produces a slower apparent rate of improvement in each single practice session but a greater accumulation of skill and learning over time.
67%
Flag icon
Interleaving. That’s a cognitive science word, and it simply means mixing related but distinct material during study.
68%
Flag icon
The mixing of items, skills, or concepts during practice, over the longer term, seems to help us not only see the distinctions between them but also to achieve a clearer grasp of each one individually.
68%
Flag icon
What made those questions so tricky, explained Rohrer, was that “math students must be able to choose a strategy—not just know how to use it—and choosing a strategy is harder when an exam covers many kinds of problems.”
70%
Flag icon
Mixing problems during study forces us to identify each type of problem and match it to the appropriate kind of solution.
70%
Flag icon
interleaving is, essentially, about preparing the brain for the unexpected.
72%
Flag icon
But they could do one thing the novices could not: memorize a chess position after seeing the board for less than five seconds.
72%
Flag icon
Their eyes, and the visual systems in their brains, are extracting the most meaningful set of clues from a vast visual tapestry, and doing so instantaneously.
72%
Flag icon
stimulus-response,
73%
Flag icon
discrimination.
74%
Flag icon
their brains came equipped with evolved modules to make important, subtle discriminations, and to put those differing symbols into categories.
74%
Flag icon
perceives to learn.
74%
Flag icon
Perceptual learning, she wrote, “is not a passive absorption, but an active process, in the sense that exploring and searching for perception itself is active.
74%
Flag icon
It’s self-correcting—it tunes itself. The system works to find the most critical perceptual signatures and filter out the rest.
75%
Flag icon
In many perceptual situations like this one, the novice is essentially blind to patterns that the expert has come to see at a glance.”
75%
Flag icon
perceptual learning module, or PLM.
78%
Flag icon
PLMs are meant for a certain kind of target: discriminating or classifying things that look the same to the untrained eye but are not.
78%
Flag icon
Run through a bunch of examples—fast—and let the sensory areas of your brain do the rest.
79%
Flag icon
sleep is essentially a time-management adaptation.
79%
Flag icon
“Another way of looking at it is that unnecessary wakefulness is a bigger mistake.”
79%
Flag icon
Sleeping and waking adjust themselves to the demands and risks of our life, not according to what the health manuals say.
79%
Flag icon
sleep’s primary purpose is memory consolidation.
81%
Flag icon
sleep architecture,
81%
Flag icon
embedded hierarchy
82%
Flag icon
“We think what’s happening during sleep is that you open the aperture of memory and are able to see this bigger picture,” the study’s senior author, Matthew Walker, told me. “There is evidence, in fact, that REM is this creative memory domain when you build different associations, combine things in different ways and so on.”
82%
Flag icon
The preponderance of evidence to date finds that sleep improves retention and comprehension of what was studied the day before,
83%
Flag icon
the Night Shift Theory.
83%
Flag icon
REM is also involved in interpreting emotionally charged memories.
83%
Flag icon
Walker describes REM as “a nighttime therapy session.”
83%
Flag icon
“Stage 2 seems to be the single most critical stage for motor learning,”
83%
Flag icon
Starve people of deep slumber, and it doesn’t just dim their beauty; they don’t get the full benefit of sleep-aided recall of newly learned facts, studied vocabulary, names, dates, and formulas. “We have a lot of evidence that slow-wave is important for declarative memory consolidation, and that this doesn’t happen as much in REM,” Stickgold told me.