Noise: A Flaw in Human Judgment
Rate it:
Open Preview
Kindle Notes & Highlights
Read between June 3 - June 18, 2021
1%
Flag icon
Bias and noise—systematic deviation and random scatter—are different components of error. The targets illustrate the difference.
1%
Flag icon
Some judgments are biased; they are systematically off target. Other judgments are noisy, as people who are expected to agree end up at very different points around the target.
1%
Flag icon
A general property of noise is that you can recognize and measure it while knowing nothing about the target or bias.
2%
Flag icon
We don’t need to know who is right to measure how much the judgments of the same case vary. All we have to do to measure noise is look at the back of the target.
2%
Flag icon
To understand error in judgment, we must understand both bias and noise. Sometimes, as we will see, noise is the more important problem.
2%
Flag icon
In real-world decisions, the amount of noise is often scandalously high. Here are a few examples of the alarming amount of noise in situations in which accuracy matters:
2%
Flag icon
Personnel decisions are noisy. Interviewers of job candidates make widely different assessments of the same people.
2%
Flag icon
Wherever you look at human judgments, you are likely to find noise. To improve the quality of our judgments, we need to overcome noise as well as bias.
3%
Flag icon
wherever there is judgment, there is noise—and more of it than you think.
7%
Flag icon
“Other people view the world much the way I do.” These beliefs, which have been called naive realism, are essential to the sense of a reality we share with other people.
8%
Flag icon
From the perspective of noise reduction, a singular decision is a recurrent decision that happens only once.
9%
Flag icon
“The personal experiences that made you who you are are not truly relevant to this decision.”
9%
Flag icon
Judgment can therefore be described as measurement in which the instrument is a human mind.
9%
Flag icon
Judgment is not a synonym for thinking, and making accurate judgments is not a synonym for having good judgment.
9%
Flag icon
A matter of judgment is one with some uncertainty about the answer and where we allow for the possibility that reasonable and competent people might disagree.
9%
Flag icon
Judges at wine competitions differ greatly on which wines should get medals, but are often unanimous in their contempt for the rejects.
19%
Flag icon
This behavior reflects a cognitive bias known as the gambler’s fallacy: we tend to underestimate the likelihood that streaks will occur by chance.
23%
Flag icon
“As I always suspected, ideas about politics and economics are a lot like movie stars. If people think that other people like them, such ideas can go far.”
26%
Flag icon
“People believe they capture complexity and add subtlety when they make judgments. But the complexity and the subtlety are mostly wasted—usually they do not add to the accuracy of simple models.”
30%
Flag icon
Overconfidence is one of the best-documented cognitive biases.
30%
Flag icon
“The average expert was roughly as accurate as a dart-throwing chimpanzee.”
30%
Flag icon
A more precise statement of the book’s message was that experts who make a living “commenting or offering advice on political and economic trends” were not “better than journalists or attentive readers of the New York Times in ‘reading’ emerging situations.”
31%
Flag icon
Tetlock’s findings suggest that detailed long-term predictions about specific events are simply impossible. The world is a messy place, where minor events can have large consequences.
32%
Flag icon
“Wherever there is prediction, there is ignorance, and probably more of it than we think. Have we checked whether the experts we trust are more accurate than dart-throwing chimpanzees?”
33%
Flag icon
“Researchers must reconcile the idea that they understand life trajectories with the fact that none of the predictions were very accurate.”
38%
Flag icon
“We know we have psychological biases, but we should resist the urge to blame every error on unspecified ‘biases.’”
38%
Flag icon
“Prejudgments and other conclusion biases lead people to distort evidence in favor of their initial position.”
49%
Flag icon
In the absence of true values to determine who is right or wrong, we often value the opinion of respect-experts even when they disagree with one another.
51%
Flag icon
“Intelligence is only part of the story, however. How people think is also important. Perhaps we should pick the most thoughtful, open-minded person, rather than the smartest one.”
53%
Flag icon
“Do you know what specific bias you’re fighting and in what direction it affects the outcome? If not, there are probably several biases at work, and it is hard to predict which one will dominate.
56%
Flag icon
“We have more information about this case, but let’s not tell the experts everything we know before they make their judgment, so as not to bias them. In fact, let’s tell them only what they absolutely need to know.” “The second opinion is not independent if the person giving it knows what the first opinion was. And the third one, even less so: there can be a bias cascade.”
58%
Flag icon
“What makes them so good is less what they are than what they do—the hard work of research, the careful thought and self-criticism, the gathering and synthesizing of other perspectives, the granular judgments and relentless updating.”
74%
Flag icon
“It might be costly to remove noise—but the cost is often worth incurring. Noise can be horribly unfair. And if one effort to reduce noise is too crude—if we end up with guidelines or rules that are unacceptably rigid or that inadvertently produce bias—we shouldn’t just give up. We have to try again.”
76%
Flag icon
“If you want to deter misconduct, you should tolerate some noise. If students are left wondering about the penalty for plagiarism, great—they will avoid plagiarizing. A little uncertainty in the form of noise can magnify deterrence.”
76%
Flag icon
“Creative people need space. People aren’t robots. Whatever your job, you deserve some room to maneuver. If you’re hemmed in, you might not be noisy, but you won’t have much fun and you won’t be able to bring your original ideas to bear.”
78%
Flag icon
If a rule is perfect, of course, it will produce no errors. But rules are rarely perfect.
78%
Flag icon
“Rules simplify life, and reduce noise. But standards allow people to adjust to the particulars of the situations.”
81%
Flag icon
The goal of judgment is accuracy, not individual expression.
81%
Flag icon
Think statistically, and take the outside view of the case.
81%
Flag icon
Structure judgments into several independent tasks.
81%
Flag icon
Resist premature intuitions.
81%
Flag icon
Obtain independent judgments from multiple judges, then consider aggregating those judgments.
82%
Flag icon
Favor relative judgments and relative scales.