Noise: A Flaw in Human Judgment
Rate it:
Open Preview
Kindle Notes & Highlights
Read between April 6 - July 24, 2024
30%
Flag icon
In particular, judgments of one’s ability to make precise predictions, even from limited information, are notoriously overconfident. What we said of noise in predictive judgments can also be said of objective ignorance: wherever there is prediction, there is ignorance, and more of it than you think.
32%
Flag icon
“Models do better than people, but not by much. Mostly, we find mediocre human judgments and slightly better models. Still, better is good, and models are better.”
32%
Flag icon
“We may never be comfortable using a model to make these decisions—we just need the internal signal to have enough confidence. So let’s make sure we have the best possible decision process.”
33%
Flag icon
The ability to make a prediction is a measure of whether such a causal chain has indeed been identified. And correlation, the measure of predictive accuracy, is a measure of how much causation we can explain.
33%
Flag icon
There is a psychological explanation for this observation. Some events are surprising: a deadly pandemic, an attack on the Twin Towers, a star hedge fund that turns out to be a Ponzi scheme. In our personal lives as well, there are occasional shocks: falling in love with a stranger, the sudden death of a young sibling, an unexpected inheritance. Other events are actively expected, like a second-grader’s return from school at the appointed time. But most human experience falls between these two extremes. We are sometimes in a state in which we actively expect a specific event, and we are ...more
33%
Flag icon
range of things we could say without shocking you.
34%
Flag icon
Causal thinking helps us make sense of a world that is far less predictable than we think. It also explains why we view the world as far more predictable than it really is. In the valley of the normal, there are no surprises and no inconsistencies. The future seems as predictable as the past. And noise is neither heard nor seen.
34%
Flag icon
“We think we understand what is going on here, but could we have predicted it?”
34%
Flag icon
when people forecast how long it will take them to complete a project, the mean of their estimates is usually much lower than the time they will actually need. This familiar psychological bias is known as the planning fallacy.
36%
Flag icon
regardless of the true reasons for your belief, you will be inclined to accept any argument that appears to support it, even when the reasoning is wrong.
37%
Flag icon
“We know we have psychological biases, but we should resist the urge to blame every error on unspecified ‘biases.’” “When we substitute an easier question for the one we should be answering, errors are bound to occur. For instance, we will ignore the base rate when we judge probability by similarity.” “Prejudgments and other conclusion biases lead people to distort evidence in favor of their initial position.” “We form impressions quickly and hold on to them even when contradictory information comes in. This tendency is called excessive coherence.”
40%
Flag icon
Our ability to compare cases is much better than our ability to place them on a scale.
40%
Flag icon
Speaking of Matching
40%
Flag icon
“Both of us say this movie is very good, but you seem to have enjoyed it a lot less than I did. We’re using the same words, but are we using the same scale?” “We thought Season 2 of this series would be just as spectacular as Season 1. We made a matching prediction, and it was wrong.” “It is hard to remain consistent when grading these essays. Should you try ranking them instead?”
43%
Flag icon
ambiguous scales are common, which means that the punitive-damages study holds two general lessons, applicable in business, education, sports, government, and elsewhere. First, the choice of a scale can make a large difference in the amount of noise in judgments, because ambiguous scales are noisy. Second, replacing absolute judgments with relative ones, when feasible, is likely to reduce noise.
43%
Flag icon
Speaking of Scales “There is a lot of noise in our judgments. Could this be because we understand the scale differently?” “Can we agree on an anchor case that will serve as a reference point on the scale?” “To reduce noise, maybe we should replace our judgments with a ranking?”
43%
Flag icon
The main implication of this view of confidence is that subjective confidence in one’s judgment by no means guarantees accuracy. Moreover, the suppression of alternative interpretations—a well-documented process in perception—could induce what we have called the illusion of agreement (see chapter 2). If people cannot imagine possible alternatives to their conclusions, they will naturally assume that other observers must reach the same conclusion, too.
45%
Flag icon
The fact that in these studies level noise is generally not the larger component of system noise is already an important message, because level noise is the only form of noise that organizations can (sometimes) monitor without conducting noise audits.
47%
Flag icon
Causes are natural; statistics are difficult.
48%
Flag icon
As we have also noted, many judgments are not verifiable. Within certain boundaries, we cannot easily know or uncontroversially define the true value at which judgments are aiming. Underwriting and criminal sentencing fall in this category, as do wine tasting, essay grading, book and movie reviewing, and innumerable other judgments. Yet some professionals in these domains come to be called experts. The confidence we have in these experts’ judgment is entirely based on the respect they enjoy from their peers. We call them respect-experts.
48%
Flag icon
More generally, how do people who are themselves respected for the quality of their judgment decide to trust someone as an expert when there is no data to establish expertise objectively? What makes a respect-expert?
48%
Flag icon
Part of the answer is the existence of shared norms, or professional doctrine.
49%
Flag icon
Beyond a knowledge of shared norms, experience is necessary, too.
49%
Flag icon
Another characteristic of respect-experts is their ability to make and explain their judgments with confidence.
49%
Flag icon
We tend to put more trust in people who trust themselves than we do in those who show their doubts.
49%
Flag icon
The confidence heuristic points to the fact that in a group, confident people have more weight than others, even if th...
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
49%
Flag icon
The conclusion is clear. GMA contributes significantly to the quality of performance in occupations that require judgment, even within a pool of high-ability individuals. The notion that there is a threshold beyond which GMA ceases to make a difference is not supported by the evidence. This
49%
Flag icon
People of high mental ability are more likely than others to make better judgments and to be true experts, but they are also more likely to impress their peers, earn others’ trust, and become respect-experts in the absence of any reality feedback. Medieval astrologers must have been among the highest-GMA people of their time.
49%
Flag icon
cognitive reflection test
49%
Flag icon
(CRT),
50%
Flag icon
Lower CRT scores are associated with many real-world judgments and beliefs, including belief in ghosts, astrology, and extrasensory perception. The scores predict whether people will fall for blatantly inaccurate “fake news.” They are even associated with how much people will use their smartphones.
50%
Flag icon
The CRT is seen by many as one instrument to measure a broader concept: the propensity to use reflective versus impulsive thought processes. Simply put, some people like to engage in careful thought, whereas others, faced with the same problem, tend to trust their first impulses. In our terminology, the CRT can be seen as a measure of people’s propensity to rely on slow, System 2 thinking rather than on fast, System 1 thinking.
50%
Flag icon
The personality of people with excellent judgment may not fit the generally accepted stereotype of a decisive leader. People often tend to trust and like leaders who are firm and clear and who seem to know, immediately and deep in their bones, what is right. Such leaders inspire confidence. But the evidence suggests that if the goal is to reduce error, it is better for leaders (and others) to remain open to counterarguments and to know that they might be wrong. If they end up being decisive, it is at the end of a process, not at the start.
52%
Flag icon
In the United States, federal agencies must compile a formal regulatory impact analysis before they issue expensive regulations designed to clean the air or water, reduce deaths in the workplace, increase food safety, respond to public health crises, reduce greenhouse gas emissions, or increase homeland security. A dense, technical document with an unlovely name (OMB Circular A-4) and spanning nearly fifty pages sets out the requirements of the analysis. The requirements are clearly designed to counteract bias. Agencies must explain why the regulation is needed, consider both more and less ...more
52%
Flag icon
Noise, on the other hand, is unpredictable error that we cannot easily see or explain. That is why we so often neglect it—even when it causes grave damage. For this reason, strategies for noise reduction are to debiasing what preventive hygiene measures are to medical treatment: the goal is to prevent an unspecified range of potential errors before they occur.
52%
Flag icon
Correcting a well-identified bias may at least give you a tangible sense of achieving something. But the procedures that reduce noise will not. They will, statistically, prevent many errors. Yet you will never know which errors. Noise is an invisible enemy, and preventing the assault of an invisible enemy can yield only an invisible victory.
56%
Flag icon
we will focus on two noise-reduction strategies that have broad applicability. One is an application of the principle we mentioned in chapter 18: selecting better judges produces better judgments. The other is one of the most universally applicable decision hygiene strategies: aggregating multiple independent estimates. The easiest way to aggregate several forecasts is to average them. Averaging is mathematically guaranteed to reduce noise: specifically, it divides it by the square root of the number of judgments averaged. This means that if you average one hundred judgments, you will reduce ...more
56%
Flag icon
Because averaging does nothing to reduce bias, its effect on total error (MSE) depends on the proportions of bias and noise in it.
56%
Flag icon
Straight averaging is not the only way to aggregate forecasts. A select-crowd strategy, which selects the best judges according to the accuracy of their recent judgments and averages the judgments of a small number of judges (e.g., five), can be as effective as straight averaging.
58%
Flag icon
“We should strive to be in perpetual beta, like the superforecasters.
59%
Flag icon
white coat syndrome—your blood pressure goes up in doctors’ offices!”
60%
Flag icon
The medical profession is likely to rely on algorithms more and more in the future; they promise to reduce both bias and noise and to save lives and money in the process.
62%
Flag icon
Speaking of Guidelines in Medicine “Among doctors, the level of noise is far higher than we might have suspected. In diagnosing cancer and heart disease—even in reading X-rays—specialists sometimes disagree. That means that the treatment a patient gets might be a product of a lottery.” “Doctors like to think that they make the same decision whether it’s Monday or Friday or early in the morning or late in the afternoon. But it turns out that what doctors say and do might well depend on how tired they are.” “Medical guidelines can make doctors less likely to blunder at a patient’s expense. Such ...more
62%
Flag icon
You might find that the answer lies in how you used the scale—what we have called level noise. Perhaps you thought a 5 requires something truly extraordinary, whereas the other rater thought that it merely requires something unusually good.
65%
Flag icon
This variability is largely the product of pattern noise, the difference in interviewers’ idiosyncratic reactions to a given interviewee.
66%
Flag icon
Why do first impressions end up driving the outcome of a much longer interview? One reason is that in a traditional interview, interviewers are at liberty to steer the interview in the direction they see fit. They are likely to ask questions that confirm an initial impression.
66%
Flag icon
As we can often find an imaginary pattern in random data or imagine a shape in the contours of a cloud, we are capable of finding logic in perfectly meaningless answers.
66%
Flag icon
however much we would like to believe that our judgment about a candidate is based on facts, our interpretation of facts is colored by prior attitudes.
75%
Flag icon
risk-averse or risk-seeking.
78%
Flag icon
Organizations all over the world see bias as a villain. They are right. They do not see noise that way. They should.