Good Habits, Bad Habits: The Science of Making Positive Changes That Stick
Rate it:
Open Preview
2%
Flag icon
If you have ever spent time in a university setting, you quickly learn that intelligence and motivation have little to do with getting things done on a regular basis.
2%
Flag icon
we are not one single unified whole. In psychological terms, we do not have a single mind.
3%
Flag icon
I didn’t actually set out to study habit; I wanted to understand how people persist.
3%
Flag icon
With actions that could be repeated often, like recycling or taking the bus, intentions didn’t matter very much.
3%
Flag icon
We now know that it’s habit that creates persistence.
4%
Flag icon
Excuse making is a talent at which our conscious minds excel.
4%
Flag icon
The truth is that many of your virtues are already habitual.
4%
Flag icon
Habits proceed so efficiently and quietly, we think we must have consciously decided to perform them.
5%
Flag icon
The mechanics of action were not taking up space in the conscious mind. This is not the repressed, Freudian version of the unconscious, but it is another way that our minds function outside of our awareness.
6%
Flag icon
Fully 43 percent of the time, our actions are habitual, performed without conscious thought.
6%
Flag icon
one important clue to what would come next: we learned that you can make pretty much any behavior more habitual, as long as you do it the same way each time.
6%
Flag icon
What I began to realize was that habit refers to how you perform an action, not what the action is.
6%
Flag icon
the key feature of habit: it works outside of our conscious awareness.
6%
Flag icon
The habits that are really driving your behavior go largely unrecognized.
6%
Flag icon
introspection illusion.3 With this cognitive bias, we overestimate the extent to which our actions depend on our internal states.
6%
Flag icon
the belief that we do the things we do because we “will” them. It’s flattering and empowering, but it’s also false.
7%
Flag icon
The illusion arises when we fail to recognize the nonconscious habits we are following and instead introspect and unwittingly confabulate explanations for our actions.
7%
Flag icon
There is another way in which over-attribution to conscious intentions can be explained. In doing so, we reconcile ourselves to our choices. They make sense to us.
7%
Flag icon
If our noisy, egotistical consciousness takes all the credit for the actions of our silent habitual self, we’ll never learn how to properly exploit this hidden resource.
7%
Flag icon
Context pervades our understanding of habit. If the context remains stable—you keep living in the same place, you keep driving the same route to work, you keep sitting on your couch every evening—then you repeat past actions automatically.
9%
Flag icon
Psychology’s reigning preoccupation with how people think left little room for studying what people actually do.
9%
Flag icon
It looked as if we had multiple ways of drawing on neural systems, one to make initial decisions and another to persist.
9%
Flag icon
Our minds do not just consciously make initial decisions; they also respond repeatedly through habit.
9%
Flag icon
Habits are a kind of action that is relatively insensitive to rewards.
Vijay Gopal
What!?
10%
Flag icon
These developments were fascinating, especially the implication that we can do something once and it’s a decision, but if we do it many times in the same way, it becomes something totally different, even recruiting different areas of our brains.
10%
Flag icon
for repeated actions, people’s intentions and goals don’t predict what they end up doing. Instead, goals seemed important for the occasional runners in our study.
10%
Flag icon
We concluded that the speed with which repeated actions are cued by contexts is central to habits. It can keep runners heading out to the track even when they feel tired.
10%
Flag icon
When people slow down to think, anything might change. Speed of thought is a clue to how habits gain control.
11%
Flag icon
And a working definition of habit emerged: a mental association between a context cue and a response that develops as we repeat an action in that context for a reward.
11%
Flag icon
But a shorthand definition is this: automaticity in lieu of conscious motivation—automaticity that emerges as we learn from repeated responding.
11%
Flag icon
A habit turns the world around you—your context—into a trigger to act.
11%
Flag icon
The situation you’re in triggers the response from memory, and you act. It can essentially bypass your executive mind. The pleasure is in the thing getting done without your consciously lifting a finger.
11%
Flag icon
your habitual mind is diligently at work. It’s not picky about what it learns. Just give it repetition, rewards, and contexts.
11%
Flag icon
The information we learn as a habit is to some extent separated from other neural regions.
16%
Flag icon
That’s a key aspect of why habits are crucial for long-term behavioral change. Brainpower is overwhelmingly costly.
16%
Flag icon
Habits, by virtue of their location deep in the rudimentary machinery of our minds, are relatively cheap. They hum along on barely any bandwidth at all.
17%
Flag icon
When they later reported their success at restraining themselves from temptations, distraction was one of the least effective strategies. The clear winner? Stimulus control.
17%
Flag icon
Even for adults, successful self-control came from essentially covering up the marshmallow.
17%
Flag icon
(You can see how you score by going to www.goodhabitsbadhabits.org. Check out “How Much Self-Control Do I Have?”)
18%
Flag icon
participants who scored highest in self-control seldom reported resisting desires, period.
18%
Flag icon
They were living their lives in a way that hid the marshmallow almost all the time.
19%
Flag icon
High “self-controllers” achieved desired outcomes by streamlining, not struggling.
19%
Flag icon
After all, this is what, until recently, we thought the “self-control” scale measures—sheer force of will. But even in these more controlled studies, the data did not support this idea.
19%
Flag icon
It is thus mainly by establishing and maintaining stable patterns of behavior rather than by performing single acts of self-denial that self-control may be most effective.”17
19%
Flag icon
It’s hard to give up the idea that people with good “self-control” achieve so much because of their willpower and self-denial.
19%
Flag icon
Bill Gates uses a flying metaphor: “Pilots like to say that good landings are the result of good approaches.”
20%
Flag icon
Habits did something special, and with such simplicity. They had essentially replaced self-control.
Vijay Gopal
interesting experiment
20%
Flag icon
Even small changes in the cue and response forced participants to make conscious decisions and to rely on executive control and willpower.
20%
Flag icon
It’s when you stop to think that you might stray from your plans and goals.
20%
Flag icon
The good effects that we popularly ascribe to “self-control” are, it seems, more accurately captured by situational control.
« Prev 1 3