Peak: Unleashing Your Inner Champion Through Revolutionary Methods for Skill Acquisition and Performance Enhancement in Work, Sports, and Life
Rate it:
Open Preview
3%
Flag icon
The right sort of practice carried out over a sufficient period of time leads to improvement. Nothing else.
3%
Flag icon
The details about this sort of practice are drawn from a relatively new area of psychology that can be best described as “the science of expertise.” This new field seeks to understand the abilities of “expert performers,” that is, people who are among the best in the world at what they do, who have reached the very peak of performance, and I have published several academic books on the topic, including Toward a General Theory of Expertise: Prospects and Limits in 1991, The Road to Excellence in 1996, and The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance in 2006.
6%
Flag icon
But while the abilities are extraordinary, there is no mystery at all about how these people developed them. They practiced. A lot. The world-record time in the marathon wasn’t cut by 30 percent over the course of a century because people were being born with a greater talent for running long distances. Nor did the second half of the twentieth century see some sudden surge in the births of people with a gift for playing Chopin or Rachmaninoff or for memorizing tens of thousands of random digits. What the second half of the twentieth century did see was a steady increase in the amount of time ...more
7%
Flag icon
You have become a tennis player. That is, you have “learned” tennis in the traditional sense, where the goal is to reach a point at which everything becomes automatic and an acceptable performance is possible with relatively little thought, so that you can just relax and enjoy the game. At this point, even if you’re not completely satisfied with your level of play, your improvement stalls. You have mastered the easy stuff.
7%
Flag icon
Research has shown that, generally speaking, once a person reaches that level of “acceptable” performance and automaticity, the additional years of “practice” don’t lead to improvement. If anything, the doctor or the teacher or the driver who’s been at it for twenty years is likely to be a bit worse than the one who’s been doing it for only five, and the reason is that these automated abilities gradually deteriorate in the absence of deliberate efforts to improve.
8%
Flag icon
Purposeful practice has well-defined, specific goals.
8%
Flag icon
Purposeful practice is all about putting a bunch of baby steps together to reach a longer-term goal.
8%
Flag icon
Purposeful practice is focused.
8%
Flag icon
You seldom improve much without giving the task your full attention.
8%
Flag icon
Purposeful practice involves feedback.
8%
Flag icon
Purposeful practice requires getting out of one’s comfort zone. This is perhaps the most important part of purposeful practice.
9%
Flag icon
The best way to get past any barrier is to come at it from a different direction, which is one reason it is useful to work with a teacher or coach.
9%
Flag icon
In all of my years of research, I have found it is surprisingly rare to get clear evidence in any field that a person has reached some immutable limit on performance. Instead, I’ve found that people more often just give up and stop trying to improve.
10%
Flag icon
Generally speaking, meaningful positive feedback is one of the crucial factors in maintaining motivation. It can be internal feedback, such as the satisfaction of seeing yourself improve at something, or external feedback provided by others, but it makes a huge difference in whether a person will be able to maintain the consistent effort necessary to improve through purposeful practice.
10%
Flag icon
So here we have purposeful practice in a nutshell: Get outside your comfort zone but do it in a focused way, with clear goals, a plan for reaching those goals, and a way to monitor your progress. Oh, and figure out a way to maintain your motivation.
10%
Flag icon
the key to improved mental performance of almost any sort is the development of mental structures that make it possible to avoid the limitations of short-term memory and deal effectively with large amounts of information at once. Steve had done this.
14%
Flag icon
The human body has a preference for stability.
15%
Flag icon
musical training modifies the structure and function of the brain in various ways that result in an increased capacity for playing music. In other words, the most effective forms of practice are doing more than helping you learn to play a musical instrument; they are actually increasing your ability to play.
16%
Flag icon
practicing the piano as a child will lead to certain neurological advantages that you just can’t match with practice as an adult.
16%
Flag icon
developing certain parts of the brain through prolonged training can come at a cost: in many cases people who have developed one skill or ability to an extraordinary degree seem to have regressed in another area.
16%
Flag icon
The reason that most people don’t possess these extraordinary physical capabilities isn’t because they don’t have the capacity for them, but rather because they’re satisfied to live in the comfortable rut of homeostasis and never do the work that is required to get out of it. They live in the world of “good enough.” The same thing is true for all the mental activities we engage in, from writing a report to driving a car, from teaching a class to running an organization, from selling houses to performing brain surgery. We learn enough to get by in our day-to-day lives, but once we reach that ...more
17%
Flag icon
And here is the key difference between the traditional approach to learning and the purposeful-practice or deliberate-practice approaches: The traditional approach is not designed to challenge homeostasis. It assumes, consciously or not, that learning is all about fulfilling your innate potential and that you can develop a particular skill or ability without getting too far out of your comfort zone. In this view, all that you are doing with practice—indeed, all that you can do—is to reach a fixed potential. With deliberate practice, however, the goal is not just to reach your potential but to ...more
18%
Flag icon
If we look at the overall history of blindfold chess, we find that this same thing is true for most blindfold chess players throughout history. They worked to become chess masters, and they found themselves, with little or no additional effort, able to play blindfolded.
18%
Flag icon
What was surprising, however, was that neither the mid-range player nor the chess master did much better than the novice in remembering the positions of pieces arranged randomly on a board.
19%
Flag icon
When asked what they see when they are mentally examining a chess position, grandmasters do not talk about visualizing the physical chess pieces on a board as they would if they were relying on some sort of “photographic memory” of the position. This would be a “bottom-level” representation. Instead, their descriptions are much more vague, sprinkled with such terms as “lines of force” and “power.” A key thing about these representations is that they allow a chess player to encode the positions of pieces on the board in a much more efficient way than simply remembering which piece is on which ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
19%
Flag icon
Much of deliberate practice involves developing ever more efficient mental representations that you can use in whatever activity you are practicing.
19%
Flag icon
A key fact about such mental representations is that they are very “domain specific,” that is, they apply only to the skill for which they were developed.
19%
Flag icon
This explains a crucial fact about expert performance in general: there is no such thing as developing a general skill. You don’t train your memory; you train your memory for strings of digits or for collections of words or for people’s faces. You don’t train to become an athlete; you train to become a gymnast or a sprinter or a marathoner or a swimmer or a basketball player. You don’t train to become a doctor; you train to become a diagnostician or a pathologist or a neurosurgeon. Of course, some people do become overall memory experts or athletes in a number of sports or doctors with a ...more
20%
Flag icon
these representations are preexisting patterns of information—facts, images, rules, relationships, and so on—that are held in long-term memory and that can be used to respond quickly and effectively in certain types of situations. The thing all mental representations have in common is that they make it possible to process large amounts of information quickly, despite the limitations of short-term memory. Indeed, one could define a mental representation as a conceptual structure designed to sidestep the usual restrictions that short-term memory places on mental processing.
20%
Flag icon
Any relatively complicated activity requires holding more information in our heads than short-term memory allows, so we are always building mental representations of one sort or another without even being aware of it.
20%
Flag icon
What sets expert performers apart from everyone else is the quality and quantity of their mental representations.
20%
Flag icon
The main thing that sets experts apart from the rest of us is that their years of practice have changed the neural circuitry in their brains to produce highly specialized mental representations, which in turn make possible the incredible memory, pattern recognition, problem solving, and other sorts of advanced abilities needed to excel in their particular specialties.
20%
Flag icon
To study this phenomenon, I and two colleagues, Paul Ward and Mark Williams, investigated how well soccer players can predict what’s coming next from what has already happened on the field. To do this we showed them videos of real soccer matches and suddenly stopped the video when a player had just received the ball. Then we asked our subjects to predict what would happen next. Would the player with the ball keep it, attempt a shot at the goal, or pass the ball to a teammate? We found that the more accomplished players were much better at deciding what the player with the ball should do. We ...more
21%
Flag icon
John Ford
Do expert climbers come up with some of their own grips based on their particular abilities, hand size, number of fingers, etc.? They may or may not have names for these kinds of grips because if they are really idiosyncratic, they won’t be talking to other people about them—at least not much.
21%
Flag icon
when experienced climbers saw a particular hold, their brains sent a signal to their hands preparing them to form the corresponding grip—again, without conscious thought.
21%
Flag icon
For the experts we just described, the key benefit of mental representations lies in how they help us deal with information: understanding and interpreting it, holding it in memory, organizing it, analyzing it, and making decisions with it. The same is true for all experts—and most of us are experts at something, whether we realize it or not.
23%
Flag icon
mental representations in turn play a key role in deliberate practice.
24%
Flag icon
Researchers refer to this sort of writing as “knowledge transforming,” as opposed to “knowledge telling,” because the process of writing changes and adds to the knowledge that the writer had when starting out.
25%
Flag icon
The first thing Imreh did when she sight-read the piece was to develop what he called an “artistic image”—a representation of what the piece should sound like when she performed it.
26%
Flag icon
These fields have several characteristics in common. First, there are always objective ways—such as the win/loss of a chess competition or a head-to-head race—or at least semiobjective ways—such as evaluation by expert judges—to measure performance.
26%
Flag icon
Second, these fields tend to be competitive enough that performers have strong incentive to practice and improve.
26%
Flag icon
Third, these fields are generally well established, with the relevant skills having been developed over decades or even centuries.
26%
Flag icon
fourth, these fields have a subset of performers who also serve as teachers and coaches and who, over time, have developed increasingly sophisticated sets of training techniques that make p...
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
27%
Flag icon
The traditional view held that differences among individuals performing at these highest levels would be due primarily to innate talent. So differences in the amount and type of practice—in essence, differences in motivation—wouldn’t matter at this level. We were looking to see if this traditional view was wrong.
27%
Flag icon
To look for these differences, we interviewed each of the thirty student violinists in our study in great detail. We asked them about their musical histories—when they started studying music, who their teachers were, how many hours a week they spent in solitary practice at each age, what competitions they’d won, and so on. We asked them for their opinions on how important various activities were in improving their performance—practicing alone, practicing in a group, playing alone for fun, playing in a group for fun, performing solo, performing in a group, taking lessons, giving lessons, ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
28%
Flag icon
The students pretty much all agreed, for instance, that solitary practice was the most important factor in improving their performance,
28%
Flag icon
was very important to their improvement. Because their practice was so intense, they needed to recharge their batteries with a full night’s sleep—and often an afternoon nap.
28%
Flag icon
One of our most significant findings was that most factors the students had identified as being important to improvement were also seen as labor-intensive and not much fun; the only exceptions were listening to music and sleeping.
28%
Flag icon
The other crucial finding was that there was only one major difference among the three groups. This was the total number of hours that the students had devoted to solitary practice.
28%
Flag icon
the largest differences in practice time among the three groups of students had come in the preteen and teenage years.
« Prev 1 3 4