How Not to Be Wrong: The Power of Mathematical Thinking
Rate it:
Open Preview
Read between December 3 - December 9, 2020
2%
Flag icon
“Mathematics is not just a sequence of computations to be carried out by rote until your patience or stamina runs out—although it might seem that way from what you’ve been taught in courses called mathematics. Those integrals are to mathematics as weight training and calisthenics are to soccer. If you want to play soccer—I mean, really play, at a competitive level—you’ve got to do a lot of boring, repetitive, apparently pointless drills. Do professional players ever use those drills? Well, you won’t see anybody on the field curling a weight or zigzagging between traffic cones. But you do see ...more
2%
Flag icon
The Statistical Research Group (SRG), where Wald spent much of World War II, was a classified program that yoked the assembled might of American statisticians to the war effort—something like the Manhattan Project, except the weapons being developed were equations, not explosives. And the SRG was actually in Manhattan, at 401 West 118th Street in Morningside Heights, just a block away from Columbia University. The building now houses Columbia faculty apartments and some doctor’s offices, but in 1943 it was the buzzing, sparking nerve center of wartime math. At the Applied Mathematics ...more
2%
Flag icon
The mathematical talent at hand was equal to the gravity of the task. In Wallis’s words, the SRG was “the most extraordinary group of statisticians ever organized, taking into account both number and quality.” Frederick Mosteller, who would later found Harvard’s statistics department, was there. So was Leonard Jimmie Savage, the pioneer of decision theory and great advocate of the field that came to be called Bayesian statistics.* Norbert Wiener, the MIT mathematician and the creator of cybernetics, dropped by from time to time. This was a group where Milton Friedman, the future Nobelist in ...more
3%
Flag icon
The armor, said Wald, doesn’t go where the bullet holes are. It goes where the bullet holes aren’t: on the engines. Wald’s insight was simply to ask: where are the missing holes? The ones that would have been all over the engine casing, if the damage had been spread equally all over the plane? Wald was pretty sure he knew. The missing bullet holes were on the missing planes. The reason planes were coming back with fewer hits to the engine is that planes that got hit in the engine weren’t coming back. Whereas the large number of planes returning to base with a thoroughly Swiss-cheesed fuselage ...more
3%
Flag icon
Wald’s recommendations were quickly put into effect, and were still being used by the navy and the air force through the wars in Korea and Vietnam. I can’t tell you exactly how many American planes they saved, though the data-slinging descendants of the SRG inside today’s military no doubt have a pretty good idea. One thing the American defense establishment has traditionally understood very well is that countries don’t win wars just by being braver than the other side, or freer, or slightly preferred by God. The winners are usually the guys who get 5% fewer of their planes shot down, or use ...more
4%
Flag icon
We tend to teach mathematics as a long list of rules. You learn them in order and you have to obey them, because if you don’t obey them you get a C-. This is not mathematics. Mathematics is the study of things that come out a certain way because there is no other way they could possibly be. Now let’s be fair: not everything in mathematics can be made as perfectly transparent to our intuition as addition and multiplication. You can’t do calculus by common sense. But calculus is still derived from our common sense—Newton took our physical intuition about objects moving in straight lines, ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
4%
Flag icon
John von Neumann, in his 1947 essay “The Mathematician,” warned: As a mathematical discipline travels far from its empirical source, or still more, if it is a second and third generation only indirectly inspired by ideas coming from “reality” it is beset with very grave dangers. It becomes more and more purely aestheticizing, more and more purely l’art pour l’art. This need not be bad, if the field is surrounded by correlated subjects, which still have closer empirical connections, or if the discipline is under the influence of men with an exceptionally well-developed taste. But there is a ...more
5%
Flag icon
But as long as you believe there’s such a thing as too much welfare state and such a thing as too little, you know the linear picture is wrong. Some principle more complicated than “More government bad, less government good” is in effect. The generals who consulted Abraham Wald faced the same kind of situation: too little armor meant planes got shot down, too much meant the planes couldn’t fly. It’s not a question of whether adding more armor is good or bad; it could be either, depending on how heavily armored the planes are to start with. If there’s an optimal answer, it’s somewhere in the ...more
7%
Flag icon
The square in the picture is called the inscribed square; each of its corners just touches the circle, but it doesn’t extend beyond the circle’s boundary. Why do this? Because circles are mysterious and intimidating, and squares are easy. If you have before you a square whose side has length X, its area is X times X—indeed, that’s why we call the operation of multiplying a number by itself squaring! A basic rule of mathematical life: if the universe hands you a hard problem, try to solve an easier one instead, and hope the simple version is close enough to the original problem that the ...more
7%
Flag icon
So the area of the circle is trapped in between 2.83 and 3.31. Why stop there? You can stick points in between the corners of the octagon (whether inscribed or circumscribed) to make a 16-gon; after some more trigonometric figuring, that tells you that the area of the circle is in between 3.06 and 3.18. Do it again, to make a 32-gon; and again, and again, and pretty soon you have something that looks like this: Wait, isn’t that just the circle? Of course not! It’s a regular polygon with 65,536 sides. Couldn’t you tell? The great insight of Eudoxus and Archimedes was that it doesn’t matter ...more
9%
Flag icon
The reason the 0.999 . . . problem is difficult is that it brings our intuitions into conflict. We would like the sum of an infinite series to play nicely with arithmetic manipulations like the ones we carried out on the previous pages, and this seems to demand that the sum equal 1. On the other hand, we would like each number to be represented by a unique string of decimal digits, which conflicts with the claim that the same number can be called either 1 or 0.999 . . . , as we like. We can’t hold on to both of these desires at once; one must be discarded. In Cauchy’s approach, which has amply ...more
Alexander White
Two words referring to the same thing: kettle and pot
11%
Flag icon
An important rule of mathematical hygiene: when you’re field-testing a mathematical method, try computing the same thing several different ways. If you get several different answers, something’s wrong with your method. For example: the 2004 bombings at the Atocha train station in Madrid killed almost 200 people. What would be an equivalently deadly bombing at Grand Central Station? The United States has almost seven times the population of Spain. So if you think of 200 people as 0.0004% of the Spanish population, you find that an equivalent attack would kill 1,300 people in the United States. ...more
12%
Flag icon
As you can see, the fraction of heads converges inexorably toward 50% as you flip more and more coins, as if squeezed by an invisible vise. You can see the same effect in the simulations. The proportions of heads in the first group of tries, the Smalls, range from 30% to 90%. With a hundred flips at a time, the range narrows: just 40% to 60%. And with a thousand flips, the range of proportions is only 46.2% to 53.7%. Something is pushing those numbers closer and closer to 50%. That something is the cold, strong hand of the Law of Large Numbers. I won’t state that theorem precisely (though it ...more
12%
Flag icon
It’s the very same effect that makes political polls less reliable when fewer voters are polled. And it’s the same, too, for brain cancer. Small states have small sample sizes—they are thin reeds whipped around by the winds of chance, while the big states are grand old oaks that barely bend. Measuring the absolute number of brain cancer deaths is biased toward the big states; but measuring the highest rates—or the lowest ones!—puts the smallest states in the lead. That’s how South Dakota can have one of the highest rates of brain cancer death while North Dakota claims one of the lowest. It’s ...more
13%
Flag icon
The bell curve/gendarme’s hat is tall in the middle and very flat near the edges, which is to say that the farther a discrepancy is from zero, the less likely it is to be encountered. And this can be precisely quantified. If you flip N coins, the chance that you’ll end up being off by at most the square root of N from 50% heads is about 95.45%. The square root of 1,000 is about 31; indeed, eighteen of our twenty big thousand-coin trials above, or 90%, were within 31 heads of 500. If I kept playing the game, the fraction of times I ended up somewhere between 469 and 531 heads would get closer ...more
13%
Flag icon
The way the overall proportion settles down to 50% isn’t that fate favors tails to compensate for the heads that have already landed; it’s that those first ten flips become less and less important the more flips we make. If I flip the coin a thousand more times, and get about half heads, then the proportion of heads in the first 1,010 flips is also going to be close to 50%. That’s how the Law of Large Numbers works: not by balancing out what’s already happened, but by diluting what’s already happened with new data, until the past is so proportionally negligible that it can safely be forgotten.
14%
Flag icon
This is a perfect example of the soup you get into when you start reporting percentages of numbers, like net job gains, that might be either positive or negative. Wisconsin added ninety-five hundred jobs, which is good; but neighboring Minnesota, under Democratic governor Mark Dayton, added more than thirteen thousand in the same month. Texas, California, Michigan, and Massachusetts also outpaced Wisconsin’s job gains. Wisconsin had a good month, that’s true—but it didn’t contribute as many jobs as the rest of the country put together, as the Republican messaging suggested. In fact, what was ...more
17%
Flag icon
What does this mean for you, if you’re fortunate enough to have some money to invest? It means you’re best off resisting the lure of the hot new fund that made 10% over the last twelve months. Better to follow the deeply unsexy advice you’re probably sick of hearing, the “eat your vegetables and take the stairs” of financial planning: instead of hunting for a magic system or an adviser with a golden touch, put your money in a big dull low-fee index fund and forget about it. When you sink your savings into the incubated fund with the eye-popping returns, you’re like the newsletter getter who ...more
17%
Flag icon
It’s massively improbable to get hit by a lightning bolt, or to win the lottery; but these things happen to people all the time, because there are a lot of people in the world, and a lot of them buy lottery tickets, or go golfing in a thunderstorm, or both. Most coincidences lose their snap when viewed from the appropriate distance. On July 9, 2007, the North Carolina Cash 5 lottery numbers came up 4, 21, 23, 34, 39. Two days later, the same five numbers came up again. That seems highly unlikely, and it seems that way because it is. The chance of those two lottery draws matching by pure chance ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
Alexander White
Australian lottery numbers 4,5,6,7,8 are questioned as being faked
17%
Flag icon
It is very unlikely that any given set of rabbinic appellations is well matched to birth and death dates in the book of Genesis. But with so many ways of choosing the names, it’s not at all improbable that among all the choices there would be one that made the Torah look uncannily prescient. Given enough chances, finding codes is a cinch. It’s especially easy if you use Michael Drosnin’s less scientific approach to code-finding. Drosnin said of code skeptics, “When my critics find a message about the assassination of a prime minister encrypted in Moby-Dick, I’ll believe them.” McKay quickly ...more
18%
Flag icon
The more chances you give yourself to be surprised, the higher your threshold for surprise had better be. If a random Internet stranger who eliminated all North American grains from his food intake reports that he dropped fifteen pounds and his eczema went away, you shouldn’t take that as powerful evidence in favor of the maize-free plan. Somebody’s selling a book about that plan, and thousands of people bought that book and tried it, and the odds are very good that, by chance alone, one among them will experience some weight loss and clear skin the next week. And that’s the guy who’s going to ...more
19%
Flag icon
The computation tells us there are exactly two times when the missile is at ground level. One time is 0.4939 . . . seconds ago. That’s when the missile was launched. The other time is 40.4939 . . . seconds from now. That’s when the missile lands. Perhaps it doesn’t seem so troubling, especially if you’re used to the quadratic formula, to get two answers to the same question. But when you’re twelve it represents a real philosophical shift. You’ve spent six long years in grade school figuring out what the answer is, and now, suddenly, there is no such thing. And those are just quadratic ...more
19%
Flag icon
This is a cubic equation, which is to say it involves x raised to the third power. Fortunately, there is a cubic formula that allows you to figure out, by a direct computation, what values of x could have gone in the box to make 12 fall out when you turn the crank. But you didn’t learn the cubic formula in school, and the reason you didn’t learn it in school is that it’s kind of a mess, and wasn’t worked out until the late Renaissance, when itinerant algebraists roamed across Italy, engaging one another in fierce public equation-solving battles with money and status on the line. The few people ...more
19%
Flag icon
If you’re the researcher who developed the new drug, the null hypothesis is the thing that keeps you up at night. Unless you can rule it out, you don’t know whether you’re on the trail of a medical breakthrough or just barking up the wrong metabolic pathway. So how do you rule it out? The standard framework, called the null hypothesis significance test, was developed in its most commonly used form by R. A. Fisher, the founder of the modern practice of statistics,* in the early twentieth century.
20%
Flag icon
So here’s the procedure for ruling out the null hypothesis, in executive bullet-point form: Run an experiment. Suppose the null hypothesis is true, and let p be the probability (under that hypothesis) of getting results as extreme as those observed. The number p is called the p-value. If it is very small, rejoice; you get to say your results are statistically significant. If it is large, concede that the null hypothesis has not been ruled out. How small is “very small”? There’s no principled way to choose a sharp dividing line between what is significant and what is not; but there’s a ...more
21%
Flag icon
A significance test is a scientific instrument, and like any other instrument, it has a certain degree of precision. If you make the test more sensitive—by increasing the size of the studied population, for example—you enable yourself to see ever-smaller effects. That’s the power of the method, but also its danger. The truth is, the null hypothesis, if we take it literally, is probably just about always false. When you drop a powerful drug into a patient’s bloodstream, it’s hard to believe the intervention has exactly zero effect on the probability that the patient will develop esophageal ...more
22%
Flag icon
He had what basketball fans call “the hot hand”—the apparent inability to miss a shot, no matter how great the distance or how fierce the defense. Except there’s supposed to be no such thing. In 1985, in one of the most famous contemporary papers in cognitive psychology, Thomas Gilovich, Robert Vallone, and Amos Tversky (hereafter GVT) did to basketball fans what B. F. Skinner had done to lovers of the Bard. They obtained records of every shot taken by the 1980−81 Philadelphia 76ers in their forty-eight home games and analyzed them statistically. If players tended toward hot streaks and cold ...more
24%
Flag icon
Joseph Berkson, the longtime head of the medical statistics division at the Mayo Clinic, who cultivated (and loudly broadcast) a vigorous skepticism about methodology he thought shaky, offered a famous example demonstrating the pitfalls of the method. Suppose you have a group of fifty experimental subjects, who you hypothesize (H) are human beings. You observe (O) that one of them is an albino. Now, albinism is extremely rare, affecting no more than one in twenty thousand people. So given that H is correct, the chance you’d find an albino among your fifty subjects is quite small, less than 1 ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
24%
Flag icon
One of the first theorems ever proved in number theory is that of Euclid, which tells us that the primes are infinite in number; we will never run out, no matter how far along the number line we let our minds range. But mathematicians are greedy types, not inclined to be satisfied with a mere assertion of infinitude. After all, there’s infinite and then there’s infinite. There are infinitely many powers of 2, but they’re very rare. Among the first one thousand numbers, there are only ten of them: 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, 32, 64, 128, 256, and 512. There are infinitely many even numbers, too, but ...more
26%
Flag icon
Modern medicine and social science are not haruspicy. But a steadily louder drum circle of dissident scientists has been pounding out an uncomfortable message in recent years: there’s probably a lot more entrail reading in the sciences than we’d like to admit. The loudest drummer is John Ioannidis, a Greek high school math star turned biomedical researcher whose 2005 paper “Why Most Published Research Findings Are False” touched off a fierce bout of self-criticism (and a second wave of self-defense) in the clinical sciences. Some papers plead for attention with a title more dramatic than the ...more
26%
Flag icon
Most scientific studies don’t consist of blundering around the genome at random; they test hypotheses that the researchers have some preexisting reason to think might be true, so the bottom row of the box is not quite so enormously dominant over the top. But the crisis of replicability is real. In a 2012 study, scientists at the California biotech company Amgen set out to replicate some of the most famous experimental results in the biology of cancer, fifty-three studies in all. In their independent trials, they were able to reproduce only six. How can this have happened? It’s not because ...more
27%
Flag icon
That slope is the shape of p-hacking. It tells you that a lot of experimental results that belong over on the unpublishable side of the p= .05 boundary have been cajoled, prodded, tweaked, or just plain tortured until, at last, they end up just on the happy side of the line. That’s good for the scientists who need publications, but it’s bad for science.
28%
Flag icon
But the culture is changing. Reformers with loud voices like Ioannidis and Simonsohn, who speak both to the scientific community and to the broader public, have generated a new sense of urgency about the danger of descent into large-scale haruspicy. In 2013, the Association for Psychological Science announced that they would start publishing a new genre of article, called Registered Replication Reports. These reports, aimed at reproducing the effects reported in widely cited studies, are treated differently from usual papers in a crucial way: the proposed experiment is accepted for publication ...more
28%
Flag icon
But some problems are more like predicting the weather. That’s another situation where having plenty of fine-grained data, and the computational power to plow through it quickly, can really help. In 1950, it took the early computer ENIAC twenty-four hours to simulate twenty-four hours of weather, and that was an astounding feat of space-age computation. In 2008, the computation was reproduced on a Nokia 6300 mobile phone in less than a second. Forecasts aren’t just faster now; they’re longer-range and more accurate, too. In 2010, a typical five-day forecast was as accurate as a three-day ...more
29%
Flag icon
It’s tempting to imagine that predictions will just get better and better as our ability to gather data gets more and more powerful; won’t we eventually have the whole atmosphere simulated to a high precision in a server farm somewhere under The Weather Channel’s headquarters? Then, if you wanted to know next month’s weather, you could just let the simulation run a little bit ahead. It’s not going to be that way. Energy in the atmosphere burbles up very quickly from the tiniest scales to the most global, with the effect that even a minuscule change at one place and time can lead to a vastly ...more
29%
Flag icon
Then the math starts. Do the terrorists tend to make more status updates per day than the general population, or fewer, or on this metric do they look basically the same? Are there words that appear more frequently in their updates? Bands or teams or products they’re unusually prone or disinclined to like? Putting all this stuff together, you can assign to each user a score,* which represents your best estimate for the probability that the user has ties, or will have ties, to terrorist groups. It’s more or less the same thing Target does when they cross-reference your lotion and vitamin ...more
34%
Flag icon
The attraction of lotteries is no novelty. The practice dates back to seventeenth-century Genoa, where it seems to have evolved by accident from the electoral system. Every six months, two of the city’s governatori were drawn from the members of the Petty Council. Rather than hold an election, Genoa carried out the election by lot, drawing two slips from a pile containing the names of all 120 councilors. Before long, the city’s gamblers began to place extravagant side bets on the election outcome. The bets became so popular that gamblers started to chafe at having to wait until
34%
Flag icon
Election Day for their enjoyable game of chance; and they quickly realized that if they wanted to bet on paper slips drawn from a pile, there was no need for an election at all. Numbers replaced names of politicians, and by 1700 Genoa was running a lottery that would look very familiar to modern Powerball players. Bettors tried to guess five randomly drawn numbers, with a bigger payoff the more numbers a player matched. Lotteries quickly spread throughout Europe, and from there to North America. During the Revolutionary War, both the Continental Congress and the governments of the states ...more
39%
Flag icon
Almost 90% of the tickets for the drawing were held by Harvey’s team. They were standing in front of the money spigot, all alone. And when the drawing was over, Random Strategies had made $700,000 on their $1.4 million investment, a cool 50% profit. This trick wasn’t going to work twice. Once the lottery realized what had happened, they put an early-warning system in place to notify top management if it looked like one of the teams was trying to push the jackpot over the roll-down line single-handedly. When Random Strategies tried again in late December, the lottery was ready. On the morning ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
40%
Flag icon
James Harvey wasn’t the first person to take advantage of a poorly designed state lottery. Gerald Selbee’s group made millions on Michigan’s original WinFall game before the state got wise and shut it down in 2005. And the practice goes back much further. In the early eighteenth century, France financed government spending by selling bonds, but the interest rate they offered wasn’t enticing enough to drive sales. To spice the pot, the government attached a lottery to the bond sales. Every bond gave its holder the right to buy a ticket for a lottery with a 500,000-livre prize, enough money to ...more
40%
Flag icon
One person who figured this out was the mathematician and explorer Charles-Marie de La Condamine; just as Harvey would do almost three centuries later, he gathered his friends into a ticket-buying cartel. One of these was the young writer François-Marie Arouet, better known as Voltaire. While he might not have contributed to the mathematics of the scheme, Voltaire placed his stamp on it. Lottery players were to write a motto on their ticket, to be read aloud when a ticket won the jackpot; Voltaire, characteristically, saw this as a perfect opportunity to epigrammatize, writing cheeky slogans ...more
41%
Flag icon
One of the first people to think clearly about expected value was Blaise Pascal; puzzled by some questions posed to him by the gambler Antoine Gombaud (self-styled the Chevalier de Méré), Pascal spent half of 1654 exchanging letters with Pierre de Fermat, trying to understand which bets, repeated over and over, would tend to be profitable in the long run, and which would lead to ruin. In modern terminology, he wished to understand which kinds of bets had positive expected value and which kinds were negative. The Pascal-Fermat correspondence is generally thought of as marking the beginning of ...more
44%
Flag icon
For Ellsberg, the answer to the paradox is simply that expected utility theory is incorrect. As Donald Rumsfeld would later put it, there are known unknowns and there are unknown unknowns, and the two are to be processed differently. The “known unknowns” are like RED—we don’t know which ball we’ll get, but we can quantify the probability that the ball will be the color we want. BLACK, on the other hand, subjects the player to an “unknown unknown”—not only are we not sure whether the ball will be black, we don’t have any knowledge of how likely it is to be black. In the decision-theory ...more
Jonah Bourne liked this
45%
Flag icon
What we’re up against here is the dreaded phenomenon known by computer-science types as “the combinatorial explosion.” Put simply: very simple operations can change manageably large numbers into absolutely impossible ones. If you want to know which of the fifty states is the most advantageous place to site your business, that’s easy; you just have to compare fifty different things. But if you want to know which route through the fifty states is the most efficient—the so-called traveling salesman problem—the combinatorial explosion goes off, and you face difficulty on a totally different scale. ...more
Jonah Bourne liked this
46%
Flag icon
How exactly Florentine artists like Filippo Brunelleschi came to develop the modern theory of perspective has occasioned a hundred quarrels among art historians, into which we won’t enter here. What we know for sure is that the breakthrough joined aesthetic concerns with new ideas from mathematics and optics. A central point was the understanding that the images we see are produced by rays of light that bounce off objects and subsequently strike our eye. This sounds obvious to a modern ear, but believe me, it wasn’t obvious then. Many of the ancient scientists, most famously Plato, argued that ...more
48%
Flag icon
The Mars orbiter Mariner 9 sent pictures of the Martian surface back to Earth using one such code, the Hadamard code. Compact discs are encoded with the Reed-Solomon code, which is why you can scratch them and they still sound perfect. (Readers born after, say, 1990 who are unfamiliar with compact discs can just think of flash drives, which use among other things the similar Bose-Chaudhuri-Hocquenghem codes to avoid data corruption.) Your bank’s routing number is encoded using a simple code called a checksum. This one is not quite an error-correcting code, but merely an error-detecting code ...more
49%
Flag icon
For a code to be an error-correcting code, no string—no point, if we’re to take this geometric analogy seriously—can be within distance 1 of two different code words; in other words, we ask that no two of the Hamming spheres centered at the code words share any points. So the problem of constructing error-correcting codes has the same structure as a classical geometric problem, that of sphere packing: how do we fit a bunch of equal-sized spheres as tightly as possible into a small space, in such a way that no two spheres overlap? More succinctly, how many oranges can you stuff into a box? The ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
49%
Flag icon
The next layer is going to look just the same as this one, but cunningly placed so that each seed sits in the little triangular divot formed by three seeds below it. Then just keep adding more layers in the same way. It’s best to be a little careful here: only half the divots are going to support spheres in the next layer up, and at each stage you have a choice of which half of the divots you want to fill. The most customary choice, called the face-centered cubic lattice, has the nice property that every layer has the spheres placed directly over the spheres three layers below. According to ...more
51%
Flag icon
So in 1933, when Secrist was ready to reveal the results of his analysis, people in both academia and business were inclined to listen. All the more so when he revealed the striking nature of his results in a 468-page volume, thickly marbled with tables and graphs. Secrist pulled no punches: he called his book The Triumph of Mediocrity in Business. “Mediocrity tends to prevail in the conduct of competitive business,” Secrist wrote. “This is the conclusion to which this study of the costs (expenses) and profits of thousands of firms unmistakably points. Such is the price which industrial ...more
This highlight has been truncated due to consecutive passage length restrictions.
52%
Flag icon
As Galton and everyone else already knew, tall parents tend to have tall children. When a six-foot-two man and a five-foot-ten woman get married, their sons and daughters are likely to be taller than average. But now here is Galton’s remarkable discovery: those children are not likely to be as tall as their parents. The same goes for short parents, in the opposite direction; their kids will tend to be short, but not as short as they themselves are. Galton had discovered the phenomenon now called regression to the mean. His data left no doubt that it was real. “However paradoxical it may appear ...more
« Prev 1