On Looking: A Walker's Guide to the Art of Observation
Rate it:
Open Preview
0%
Flag icon
It is forgotten not because nothing of interest happens. It is forgotten because we failed to pay attention to the journey to begin with.
1%
Flag icon
Even the feeling of time passing changes on our walk: with less to notice, time speeds up. The capacity to attend is ours; we just forget how to turn it on.
2%
Flag icon
I would find myself at once alarmed, delighted, and humbled at the limitations of my ordinary looking. My consolation is that this deficiency of mine is quite human. We see, but we do not see: we use our eyes, but our gaze is glancing, frivolously considering its object.
2%
Flag icon
We are not blinded, but we have blinders.
3%
Flag icon
Objects in the world may seem benign, impotently hoping that your glance will light on them, but they are competing with each other for your regard. Attention is an intentional, unapologetic discriminator. It asks what is relevant right now, and gears us up to notice only that.
4%
Flag icon
I am looking for what it is that I miss, every day, right in front of me,
5%
Flag icon
trying out different forms of locomotion (among them running, marching, high-kicking, galloping, scooting, projectile falling, spinning, and noisy shuffling). It is archeology: exploring the bit of discarded candy wrapper; collecting a fistful of pebbles and a twig and a torn corner of a paperback; swishing dirt back and forth along the ground. It is stopping to admire the murmuring of the breeze in the trees; locating the source of the bird’s song; pointing. Pointing!— using the arm to extend one’s fallen gaze so someone else can see what you’ve seen. It is a time of sharing.
5%
Flag icon
The infant’s world is a case study in confused attention.
6%
Flag icon
Synesthesia—literally “joining of sensations”—is a somewhat rare and highly improbable form of perception in adults.
7%
Flag icon
We all begin in a kind of sensory chaos—what William James called an “aboriginal sensible muchness”:
8%
Flag icon
Soon, though, we acclimate. Familiarity begins following us around. Before we know it, we have become entirely accustomed to how that vacation spot looks.
10%
Flag icon
the trash-top shoe is forlorn-looking, if not itself feeling that way. There is a richness in the child’s analogies that we lose when we learn to be obsessed with “appropriate” word use. It is a sign of smallness of mind to think of this appreciation for the shoe’s situation or the blooming flower’s emotion as an error.
10%
Flag icon
Compassion emerges from imagining the world alive. I myself felt I was losing the sensitivity to broken chairs left out on the street that I once had.
12%
Flag icon
Expertise changes what you see and hear, and it even changes what you can attend to.
14%
Flag icon
I now saw Horenstein, too, changed by his own expertise. He can never walk down a block and not see its geology. We all have our own chesslike expertise in our heads, the place we know impossibly well, the images with which we are intimately familiar, the fine motor skill or athletic grace we can recognize in other people.
15%
Flag icon
The infant vacillates between being a catastrophist and a purring kitten.
15%
Flag icon
For the first five years of life, it is said, children learn approximately one word every two hours they are awake.
16%
Flag icon
Words are the ample cleavage of the urban environment: impossible not to look at.
18%
Flag icon
packed tight with perceptual pudding
20%
Flag icon
One perceptual constraint that I knowingly labor under is the constraint that we all create for ourselves: we summarize and generalize, stop looking at particulars and start taking in scenes at a glance—all
20%
Flag icon
To the child, as to the artist, everything is relevant; little is unseen.
21%
Flag icon
She left the route and wandered
24%
Flag icon
her ability to transcend the social and cultural knowledge of where one is allowed to go felt like a superpower.
24%
Flag icon
The reduction in receptors might actually increase information flow to various parts of the brain,
24%
Flag icon
“Thinking outside the box might be facilitated by having a somewhat less intact box,” the researchers wrote.
26%
Flag icon
My mind boggled a small boggle.
33%
Flag icon
expectation magically sorts the world into things-we-are-looking-for and things-we-are-not.
34%
Flag icon
rats are thigmotaxic: a splendid word to describe an animal who likes to walk along walls, touching something as it goes. Thigmotaxic, or thigmophilic—touch-loving—animals
35%
Flag icon
Watching the birds soar, pitch, and roll, and feeling happier just observing them, it occurred to me that one of the reasons that it is hard to pinpoint the function of this behavior may be that it is functionless. And the most classic functionless behavior, seen in all mammals and most vertebrates, is play. Might these birds be soaring for the mere pleasure of it, a communal recess run to nowhere in particular?
41%
Flag icon
Sometimes we see least the things we see most.
44%
Flag icon
Philadelphia is already the color of rain, and she wore the damp comfortably.
44%
Flag icon
Gait is like the poker player’s tell: revealing of all faults.
46%
Flag icon
Efficient was how Johnson defined the perfect gait.
53%
Flag icon
our brains are also alert to “peripersonal space,” the bubble of space outlined by and directly surrounding our bodies. The bubble extends to right about where our limbs can extend—so it is larger for people with longer arms, piano-player fingers, or legs up to there. Neuroscientists discovered cells in the brains of monkeys and humans that are specialized to fire to sounds, touch, and sights in this near space.
54%
Flag icon
The urban windscape manifests a Who’s Who of physics phenomena, involving forces and flows described by the Bernoulli principle, the Venturi effect, turbulence, and the properties of eddies and vortices.
55%
Flag icon
the air being blown around in a large, wide-open area accelerates dramatically as it gets pushed into the small canyon of the street (the Venturi effect).
55%
Flag icon
Tall buildings create other wind effects: winds that hit high on a building rush down its face,
55%
Flag icon
Sheer glass towers can pull air not just down, but also up from below (the Bernoulli principle)—as
57%
Flag icon
Naming, though, is not the exclusive reason for listening. Indeed, at times naming a sound aborts the experience of hearing altogether, shutting us off from continued listening and exploring the nature of the sound. Oh, that’s just a downy woodpecker rat-a-tat-tatting. (Now we can move on.) Satisfied with our identification, we shift from attending to the downy ruckus to ruminating on dinner plans.
58%
Flag icon
If those vibrations are strong enough, the hair cells bend deeply under their force. Air pressure can mow, crush, or sever the hairs until they are splayed, fused, floppy, or fractured—an
58%
Flag icon
Bent and damaged enough because of exposure to loud sounds for prolonged periods, the hair cells do not grow back; the ears lose their neural downiness. The world becomes progressively quieter for the person attached to those ears, until there are no sounds, no music, no noise.
58%
Flag icon
The force required to move our dancing ear hairs is so minimal that it would disrupt the tiniest mosquito not at all. Unlike our other senses, in listening, the normally functioning ear actually makes its own noise, something called otoacoustic emissions.
61%
Flag icon
There is a tactile side to sound. More so than with vision, we can experience the physical agent (sound waves) in two ways at once.
61%
Flag icon
Once light becomes “ultraviolet” and invisible to us, we can feel it, but it is the feeling of our tissues slowly burning.
61%
Flag icon
There are other cross-modal components to listening. It is visual, for one thing. Close your eyes, and your hearing is more focused.
63%
Flag icon
different temperatures control how far and where sounds will travel.
63%
Flag icon
evolution favors those who intuitively know how to best send a sound signal through a medium—say, air or water.
65%
Flag icon
In the back of the nose is an “olfactory recess” separated from the main respiratory pathway by a bony plate, allowing smelling to be distinct from breathing, and letting odors loiter for a long time to be considered.
66%
Flag icon
One out of every fifty genes is committed to making cells that can detect smells. So smells are important to us.
66%
Flag icon
Dogs detect odors at one or two parts per trillion, unimaginably more sensitive than we are.
« Prev 1