The Righteous Mind: Why Good People are Divided by Politics and Religion
Rate it:
Open Preview
1%
Flag icon
“Please, we can get along here. We all can get along. I mean, we’re all stuck here for a while. Let’s try to work it out.”
1%
Flag icon
Part I is about the first principle: Intuitions come first, strategic reasoning second.7 Moral intuitions arise automatically and almost instantaneously, long before moral reasoning has a chance to get started, and those first intuitions tend to drive our later reasoning.
1%
Flag icon
The central metaphor of these four chapters is that the mind is divided, like a rider on an elephant, and the rider’s job is to serve the elephant. The rider is our conscious reasoning—the stream of words and images of which we are fully aware. The elephant is the other 99 percent of mental processes—the ones that occur outside of awareness but that actually govern most of our behavior.
1%
Flag icon
Part II is about the second principle of moral psychology, which is that there’s more to morality than harm and fairness. The central metaphor of these four chapters is that the righteous mind is like a tongue with six taste receptors.
2%
Flag icon
is about the third principle: Morality binds and blinds. The central metaphor of these four chapters is that human beings are 90 percent chimp and 10 percent bee.
2%
Flag icon
Why do you see the speck in your neighbor’s eye, but do not notice the log in your own eye?… You hypocrite, first take the log out of your own eye, and then you will see clearly to take the speck out of your neighbor’s eye. (MATTHEW 7:3–5)
2%
Flag icon
The Perfect Way is only difficult                 for those who pick and choose;                 Do not like, do not dislike;
2%
Flag icon
all will then be clear.                 Make a hairbreadth difference,                 and Heaven and Earth are set apart;                 If you want the truth to stand clear before you,                 never be for or against.                 The struggle between “for” and “against”                 is the mind’s worst disease.11
2%
Flag icon
The mind is divided, like a rider on an elephant, and the rider’s job is to serve the elephant.
2%
Flag icon
A family’s dog was killed by a car in front of their house. They had heard that dog meat was delicious, so they cut up the dog’s body and cooked it and ate it for dinner. Nobody saw them do this.
2%
Flag icon
A man goes to the supermarket once a week and buys a chicken. But before cooking the chicken, he has sexual intercourse with it. Then he cooks it and eats it.
2%
Flag icon
But now the disgust is so much stronger, and the action just seems so … degrading. Does that make it wrong?
2%
Flag icon
But if you are not a liberal or libertarian Westerner, you probably think it’s wrong—morally wrong—for someone to have sex with a chicken carcass and then eat it.
3%
Flag icon
Where does morality come from? There are two obvious answers to this question: nature or nurture.
3%
Flag icon
in 1987 moral psychology was mostly focused on a third answer: rationalism, which says that kids figure out morality for themselves.
3%
Flag icon
In other words, the understanding of the conservation of volume wasn’t innate, and it wasn’t learned from adults. Kids figure it out for themselves, but only when their minds are ready and they are given the right kinds of experiences.
3%
Flag icon
This growing knowledge came in orderly stages, as children’s cognitive abilities matured. Piaget argued that children’s understanding of morality is like their understanding of those water glasses: we can’t say that it is innate, and we can’t say that kids learn it directly from adults.6 It is, rather, self-constructed as kids play with other kids.
3%
Flag icon
This is the essence of psychological rationalism: We grow into our rationality as caterpillars grow into butterflies.
3%
Flag icon
if the child gets enough experiences of turn taking, sharing, and playground justice, it will (eventually) become a moral creature, able to use its rational capacities to solve ever harder problems.
3%
Flag icon
Lawrence Kohlberg,
3%
Flag icon
First, he developed a way to quantify Piaget’s observation that children’s moral reasoning changed over time.
3%
Flag icon
Kohlberg found a six-stage progression in children’s reasoning about the social world, and this progression matched up well with the stages Piaget had found in children’s reasoning
3%
Flag icon
about the physical world.
3%
Flag icon
Kohlberg called the first two stages the “pre-conventional” level of moral judgment, and they correspond to the Piagetian stage at which kids judge the physical world by superficial features (if a glass is taller, then it has more water in it).
3%
Flag icon
during elementary school, most children move on to the two “conventional” stages, becoming adept at understanding and even manipulating rules and social conventions.
3%
Flag icon
After puberty, right when Piaget said that children become capable of abstract thought, Kohlberg found that some children begin to think for themselves about the nature of authority, the meaning of justice, and the reasons behind rules and laws.
3%
Flag icon
Kohlberg’s second great innovation: he used his research to build a scientific justification for a secular liberal moral order.
3%
Flag icon
Kohlberg’s most influential finding was that the most morally advanced kids (according to his scoring technique) were those who had frequent opportunities for role taking—for putting themselves into another person’s shoes and looking at a problem from that person’s perspective.
4%
Flag icon
Piaget and Kohlberg both thought that parents and other authorities were obstacles to moral development.
4%
Flag icon
Elliot Turiel developed such a technique. His innovation was to tell children short stories about other kids who break rules
4%
Flag icon
and then give them a series of simple yes-or-no probe questions.
4%
Flag icon
Children recognize that rules about clothing, food, and many other aspects of life are social conventions, which are arbitrary and changeable to some extent.
4%
Flag icon
Children recognize that rules that prevent harm are moral rules, which Turiel defined as rules related to “justice, rights, and welfare pertaining to how people ought to relate to each other.”13
4%
Flag icon
Children construct their moral understanding on the bedrock of the absolute moral truth that harm is wrong.
4%
Flag icon
morality is about treating individuals well. It’s about harm and fairness (not loyalty, respect, duty, piety, patriotism, or tradition).
4%
Flag icon
The Azande believed that witches were just as likely to be men as women, and the fear of being called a witch made the Azande careful not to make their neighbors angry or envious. That was my first hint that groups create supernatural beings not to explain the universe but to order their societies.
4%
Flag icon
tribe in the Philippines whose young men gained honor by cutting off people’s heads.
4%
Flag icon
The author explained these most puzzling killings as ways that small groups of men channeled resentments and frictions within the group into a group-strengthening “hunting party,” capped off by a long night of communal celebratory singing. This was my first hint that morality often involves tension within the group linked to competition between different groups.
5%
Flag icon
If Turiel was right that morality is really about harm, then why do most non-Western cultures moralize so many
5%
Flag icon
practices that seem to have nothing to do with harm?
5%
Flag icon
Liberals sometimes say that religious conservatives are sexual prudes for whom anything other than missionary-position intercourse within marriage is a sin. But conservatives can just as well make fun of liberal struggles to choose a balanced breakfast—balanced among moral concerns about free-range eggs, fair-trade coffee, naturalness, and a variety of toxins, some of which (such as genetically modified corn and soybeans) pose a greater threat spiritually than biologically.
5%
Flag icon
The Western conception of the person as a bounded, unique, more or less integrated motivational and cognitive universe, a dynamic center of awareness, emotion, judgment, and action organized into a distinctive whole and set contrastively both against other such wholes and against its social and natural background, is, however incorrigible it may seem to us, a rather peculiar idea within the context of the world’s cultures.
5%
Flag icon
the most important being how to balance the needs of individuals and groups. There seem to be just two primary ways of answering this question. Most societies have chosen the sociocentric answer, placing the needs of groups and institutions first, and subordinating the needs of individuals. In contrast, the individualistic answer places individuals at the center and makes society a servant of the individual.
5%
Flag icon
Shweder thought that the theories of Kohlberg and Turiel were produced by and for people from individualistic cultures. He doubted that those theories would apply in Orissa, where morality was sociocentric, selves were interdependent, and no bright line separated moral rules (preventing harm) from social conventions (regulating behaviors not linked directly to harm).
5%
Flag icon
But the Indians would not condemn other cases that seemed (to Americans) just as clearly to involve harm and unfairness (see middle third).
5%
Flag icon
Indian practices related to food, sex, clothing, and gender relations were almost always judged to be moral issues, not social conventions, and there were few differences between the adults and children within each city.
5%
Flag icon
Shweder found almost no trace of social conventional thinking in the sociocentric culture of Orissa, where, as he put it, “the social order is a moral order.”
5%
Flag icon
Actions that Indians and Americans agreed were wrong: • While walking, a man saw a dog sleeping on the road. He walked up to it and kicked it. • A father said to his son, “If you do well on the exam, I will buy you a pen.” The son did well on the exam, but the father did not give him anything. Actions that Americans said were wrong but Indians said were acceptable: • A young married woman went alone to see a movie without informing her husband. When she returned home her husband said, “If you do it again, I will beat you black and blue.” She did it again; he beat her black and blue. (Judge the ...more
5%
Flag icon
her clothes before cooking.
6%
Flag icon
Even in Chicago, Shweder found relatively little evidence of social-conventional thinking. There were plenty of stories that contained no obvious harm or injustice, such as a widow eating fish, and Americans predictably said that those cases were fine. But more important, they didn’t see these behaviors as social conventions that could be changed by popular consent.
« Prev 1 3 12