Thinking, Fast and Slow
Rate it:
Open Preview
Read between March 4 - July 28, 2023
16%
Flag icon
The standard practice of open discussion gives too much weight to the opinions of those who speak early and assertively, causing others to line up behind them.
16%
Flag icon
An essential design feature of the associative machine is that it represents only activated ideas. Information that is not retrieved (even unconsciously) from memory might as well not exist.
19%
Flag icon
The mental shotgun makes it easy to generate quick answers to difficult questions without imposing much hard work on your lazy System 2.
19%
Flag icon
The automatic processes of the mental shotgun and intensity matching often make available one or more answers to easy questions that could be mapped onto the target question.
19%
Flag icon
System 2 has the opportunity to reject this intuitive answer, or to modify it by incorporating other information. However, a lazy System 2 often follows the path of least effort and endorses a heuristic answer without much scrutiny of whether it is truly appropriate.
19%
Flag icon
a judgment that is based on substitution will inevitably be biased in predictable ways. In this case, it happens so deep in the perceptual system that you simply cannot help it.
20%
Flag icon
The dominance of conclusions over arguments is most pronounced where emotions are involved.
20%
Flag icon
System 1 is highly adept in one form of thinking—it automatically and effortlessly identifies causal connections between events, sometimes even when the connection is spurious.
21%
Flag icon
psychologists commonly chose samples so small that they exposed themselves to a 50% risk of failing to confirm their true hypotheses! No researcher in his right mind would accept such a risk. A plausible explanation was that psychologists’ decisions about sample size reflected prevalent intuitive misconceptions of the extent of sampling variation.
21%
Flag icon
“people are not adequately sensitive to sample size.”
21%
Flag icon
System 1 is not prone to doubt. It suppresses ambiguity and spontaneously constructs stories that are as coherent as possible. Unless the message is immediately negated, the associations that it evokes will spread as if the message were true. System 2 is capable of doubt, because it can maintain incompatible possibilities at the same time. However, sustaining doubt is harder work than sliding into certainty. The law of small numbers is a manifestation of a general bias that favors certainty over doubt, which will turn up in many guises in following chapters.
21%
Flag icon
we are prone to exaggerate the consistency and coherence of what we see.
21%
Flag icon
System 1 runs ahead of the facts in constructing a rich image on the basis of scraps of evidence.
21%
Flag icon
The difficulty we have with statistical regularities is that they call for a different approach. Instead of focusing on how the event at hand came to be, the statistical view relates it to what could have happened instead. Nothing in particular caused it to be what it is—chance selected it from among its alternatives.
21%
Flag icon
We do not expect to see regularity produced by a random process, and when we detect what appears to be a rule, we quickly reject the idea that the process is truly random.
22%
Flag icon
The tendency to see patterns in randomness is overwhelming—certainly
22%
Flag icon
We are far too willing to reject the belief that much of what we see in life is random.
22%
Flag icon
anchoring effect. It occurs when people consider a particular value for an unknown quantity before estimating that quantity.
22%
Flag icon
adjust-and-anchor heuristic as a strategy for estimating uncertain quantities: start from an anchoring number, assess whether it is too high or too low, and gradually adjust your estimate by mentally “moving” from the anchor. The adjustment typically ends prematurely, because people stop when they are no longer certain that they should move farther.
23%
Flag icon
adjustment is a deliberate attempt to find reasons to move away from the anchor:
23%
Flag icon
System 1 tries its best to construct a world in which the anchor is the true number.
24%
Flag icon
a strategy of deliberately “thinking the opposite” may be a good defense against anchoring effects, because it negates the biased recruitment of thoughts that produces these effects.
24%
Flag icon
Whether the story is true, or believable, matters little, if at all.
24%
Flag icon
The main moral of priming research is that our thoughts and our behavior are influenced, much more than we know or want, by the environment of the moment.
24%
Flag icon
you should assume that any number that is on the table has had an anchoring effect on you, and if the stakes are high you should mobilize yourself (your System 2) to combat the effect.
24%
Flag icon
availability heuristic as the process of judging frequency by “the ease with which instances come to mind.”
24%
Flag icon
you wish to estimate the size of a category or the frequency of an event, but you report an impression of the ease with which instances come to mind.
25%
Flag icon
The experience of fluent retrieval of instances trumped the number retrieved.
26%
Flag icon
The world in our heads is not a precise replica of reality; our expectations about the frequency of events are distorted by the prevalence and emotional intensity of the messages to which we are exposed.
26%
Flag icon
affect heuristic, in which people make judgments and decisions by consulting their emotions: Do I like it? Do I hate it? How strongly do I feel about it?
26%
Flag icon
people who do not display the appropriate emotions before they decide, sometimes because of brain damage, also have an impaired ability to make good decisions. An inability to be guided by a “healthy fear” of bad consequences is a disastrous flaw.
26%
Flag icon
The affect heuristic simplifies our lives by creating a world that is much tidier than reality. Good technologies have few costs in the imaginary world we inhabit, bad technologies have no benefits, and all decisions are easy. In the real world, of course, we often face painful tradeoffs between benefits and costs.
26%
Flag icon
“Risk” does not exist “out there,” independent of our minds and culture, waiting to be measured. Human beings have invented the concept of “risk” to help them understand and cope with the dangers and uncertainties of life. Although these dangers are real, there is no such thing as “real risk” or “objective risk.”
27%
Flag icon
the importance of an idea is often judged by the fluency (and emotional charge) with which that idea comes to mind.
27%
Flag icon
a basic limitation in the ability of our mind to deal with small risks: we either ignore them altogether or give them far too much weight—nothing in between
27%
Flag icon
I share Sunstein’s discomfort with the influence of irrational fears and availability cascades on public policy in the domain of risk. However, I also share Slovic’s belief that widespread fears, even if they are unreasonable, should not be ignored by policy makers. Rational or not, fear is painful and debilitating, and policy makers must endeavor to protect the public from fear, not only from real dangers.
30%
Flag icon
The most coherent stories are not necessarily the most probable, but they are plausible, and the notions of coherence, plausibility, and probability are easily confused by the unwary.
30%
Flag icon
adding detail to scenarios makes them more persuasive, but less likely to come true.
31%
Flag icon
System 2 is not impressively alert.
31%
Flag icon
The laziness of System 2 is an important fact of life, and the observation that representativeness can block the application of an obvious logical rule is also of some interest.
31%
Flag icon
Intuition governs judgments in the between-subjects condition; logic rules in joint evaluation.
31%
Flag icon
the norms of debate in the social sciences do not prohibit the political style of argument, especially when large issues are at stake—and
32%
Flag icon
stereotypes, both correct and false, are how we think of categories.
32%
Flag icon
Resistance to stereotyping is a laudable moral position, but the simplistic idea that the resistance is costless is wrong. The costs are worth paying to achieve a better society, but denying that the costs exist, while satisfying to the soul and politically correct, is not scientifically defensible.
32%
Flag icon
The experiment shows that individuals feel relieved of responsibility when they know that others have heard the same request for help.
33%
Flag icon
some potent feature of the situation, such as the diffusion of responsibility, induces normal and decent people such as them to behave in a surprisingly unhelpful way.
33%
Flag icon
rewards for improved performance work better than punishment of mistakes.
33%
Flag icon
poor performance was typically followed by improvement and good performance by deterioration, without any help from either praise or punishment.
33%
Flag icon
Because we tend to be nice to other people when they please us and nasty when they do not, we are statistically punished for being nice and rewarded for being nasty.
35%
Flag icon
if the topic of regression comes up in a criminal or civil trial, the side that must explain regression to the jury will lose the case. Why is it so hard? The main reason for the difficulty is a recurrent theme of this book: our mind is strongly biased toward causal explanations and does not deal well with “mere statistics.” When our attention is called to an event, associative memory will look for its cause—more precisely, activation will automatically spread to any cause that is already stored in memory. Causal explanations will be evoked when regression is detected, but they will be wrong ...more