Thinking, Fast and Slow
Rate it:
Kindle Notes & Highlights
Read between April 18, 2017 - January 19, 2020
39%
Flag icon
behavior both on the test and in the real world is determined by many factors that are specific to the particular situation. Remove one highly assertive member from a group of eight candidates and everyone else’s personalities will appear to change.
39%
Flag icon
illusion of validity.”
39%
Flag icon
illusion of skill?”
39%
Flag icon
The statistical algorithm used only a fraction of this information: high school grades and one aptitude test. Nevertheless, the formula was more accurate than 11 of the 14 counselors. Meehl reported generally similar results across a variety of other forecast outcomes,
39%
Flag icon
but the score in the contest between algorithms and humans has not changed. About 60% of the studies have shown significantly better accuracy for the algorithms. The other comparisons scored a draw in accuracy,
40%
Flag icon
One reason, which Meehl suspected, is that experts try to be clever, think outside the box, and consider complex combinations of features in making their predictions. Complexity may work in the odd case, but more often than not it reduces validity.
40%
Flag icon
According to Meehl, there are few circumstances under which it is a good idea to substitute judgment for a formula.
40%
Flag icon
Another reason for the inferiority of expert judgment is that humans are incorrigibly inconsistent in making summary judgments of complex information.
40%
Flag icon
Experienced radiologists who evaluate chest X-rays as “normal” or “abnormal” contradict themselves 20% of the time when they see the same picture on separate occasions.
40%
Flag icon
suggests that this level of inconsistency is typical,
40%
Flag icon
The widespread inconsistency is probably due to the extreme context dependency of System 1. We know from studies of priming that unnoticed stimuli in our environment have a substantial influence on our thoughts and actions.
40%
Flag icon
Because you have little direct knowledge of what goes on in your mind, you will never know that you might have made a different judgment or reached a different decision under very slightly different circumstances.
40%
Flag icon
The research suggests a surprising conclusion: to maximize predictive accuracy, final decisions should be left to formulas, especially in low-validity environments.
Jon Bell
Is there an algorithm for medical/nursing hiring.
40%
Flag icon
A formula that combines these predictors with equal weights is likely to be just as accurate in predicting new cases as the multiple-regression formula that was optimal in the original sample.
40%
Flag icon
The surprising success of equal-weighting schemes has an important practical implication: it is possible to develop useful algorithms without any prior statistical research.
40%
Flag icon
The important conclusion from this research is that an algorithm that is constructed on the back of an envelope is often good enough to compete with an optimally weighted formula, and certainly good enough to outdo expert judgment.
40%
Flag icon
Until the anesthesiologist Virginia Apgar intervened in 1953, physicians and midwives used their clinical judgment to determine whether a baby was in distress. Different practitioners focused on different cues. Some watched for breathing problems while others monitored how soon the baby cried. Without a standardized procedure, danger signs were often missed, and many newborn infants died.
40%
Flag icon
Many of these hunches are confirmed, illustrating the reality of clinical skill. The problem is that the correct judgments involve short-term predictions in the context of the therapeutic interview, a skill in which therapists may have years of practice. The tasks at which they fail typically require long-term predictions about the patient’s future.
40%
Flag icon
They know they are skilled, but they don’t necessarily know the boundaries of their skill.
41%
Flag icon
Meehl and other proponents of algorithms have argued strongly that it is unethical to rely on intuitive judgments for important decisions if an algorithm is available that will make fewer mistakes.
41%
Flag icon
The story of a child dying because an algorithm made a mistake is more poignant than the story of the same tragedy occurring as a result of human error, and the difference in emotional intensity is readily translated into a moral preference.
41%
Flag icon
do not simply trust intuitive judgment—your own or that of others—but do not dismiss it, either.
42%
Flag icon
Klein elaborated this description into a theory of decision making that he called the recognition-primed decision (RPD) model, which applies to firefighters but also describes expertise in other domains, including chess.
42%
Flag icon
This strong statement reduces the apparent magic of intuition to the everyday experience of memory.
42%
Flag icon
The moral of Simon’s remark is that the mystery of knowing without knowing is not a distinctive feature of intuition; it is the norm of mental life.
42%
Flag icon
The acquisition of expertise in complex tasks such as high-level chess, professional basketball, or firefighting is intricate and slow because expertise in a domain is not a single skill but rather a large collection of miniskills.
42%
Flag icon
as he told me, true experts know the limits of their knowledge.
43%
Flag icon
The associative machine is set to suppress doubt and to evoke ideas and information that are compatible with the currently dominant story. A mind that follows WYSIATI will achieve high confidence much too easily by ignoring what it does not know.
43%
Flag icon
Some environments are worse than irregular. Robin Hogarth described “wicked” environments, in which professionals are likely to learn the wrong lessons from experience.
43%
Flag icon
Meehl’s clinicians were not inept and their failure was not due to lack of talent. They performed poorly because they were assigned tasks that did not have a simple solution.
43%
Flag icon
Statistical algorithms greatly outdo humans in noisy environments for two reasons: they are more likely than human judges to detect weakly valid cues and much more likely to maintain a modest level of accuracy by using such cues consistently.
43%
Flag icon
Remember this rule: intuition cannot be trusted in the absence of stable regularities in the environment.
43%
Flag icon
Whether professionals have a chance to develop intuitive expertise depends essentially on the quality and speed of feedback, as well as on sufficient opportunity to practice.
43%
Flag icon
Expertise is not a single skill; it is a collection of skills, and the same professional may be highly expert in some of the tasks in her domain while remaining a novice in others.
43%
Flag icon
Furthermore, some aspects of any professional’s tasks are much easier to learn than others.
43%
Flag icon
Our conclusion was that for the most part it is possible to distinguish intuitions that are likely to be valid from those that are likely to be bogus.
43%
Flag icon
Unfortunately, associative memory also generates subjectively compelling intuitions that are false.
43%
Flag icon
System 1 is often able to produce quick answers to difficult questions by substitution, creating coherence where there is none. The question that is answered is not the one that was intended, but the answer is produced quickly and may be sufficiently plausible to pass the lax and lenient review of System 2.
44%
Flag icon
The first was immediately apparent: I had stumbled onto a distinction between two profoundly different approaches to forecasting, which Amos and I later labeled the inside view and the outside view.
44%
Flag icon
The second lesson was that our initial forecasts of about two years for the completion of the project exhibited a planning fallacy.
44%
Flag icon
third lesson, which I call irrational perseverance:
44%
Flag icon
“Pallid” statistical information is routinely discarded when it is incompatible with one’s personal impressions of a case. In the competition with the inside view, the outside view doesn’t stand a chance.
44%
Flag icon
A proud emphasis on the uniqueness of cases is also common in medicine, in spite of recent advances in evidence-based medicine that point the other way.
45%
Flag icon
coined the term planning fallacy to describe plans and forecasts that are unrealistically close to best-case scenarios could be improved by consulting the statistics of similar cases
45%
Flag icon
The renowned Danish planning expert Bent Flyvbjerg, now at Oxford University, offered a forceful summary: The prevalent tendency to underweight or ignore distributional information is perhaps the major source of error in forecasting. Planners should therefore make every effort to frame the forecasting problem so as to facilitate utilizing all the distributional information that is available.
45%
Flag icon
The treatment for the planning fallacy has now acquired a technical name, reference class forecasting,
45%
Flag icon
“A budget reserve is to contractors as red meat is to lions, and they will devour it.”
45%
Flag icon
A well-run organization will reward planners for precise execution and penalize them for failing to anticipate difficulties, and for failing to allow for difficulties that they could not have anticipated—the unknown unknowns.
46%
Flag icon
One of the benefits of an optimistic temperament is that it encourages persistence in the face of obstacles. But persistence can be costly.
46%
Flag icon
However, 47% of them continued development efforts even after being told that their project was hopeless, and on average these persistent (or obstinate) individuals doubled their initial losses before giving up.
1 7 12