ريتشارد دوكنز's Blog, page 600

December 30, 2015

Debate Rages Over Whether Speaking A Second Language Improves Cognition

The idea that learning to speak two languages is good for your brain has come to be widely accept as fact, particularly in popular media. Studies have shown that bilingual speakers of all ages outperform monolinguals on certain cognitive performance measures. Other studies show delays in the onset of dementia and some even claim enhanced intelligence.


But a handful of attempts to replicate some of these seminal findings have failed to confirm this “bilingual advantage.” The number of studies that have not found a tie between bilingualism and better cognition has risen dramatically over the past few years.


A heated debate over this issue now rages in the research community and has gained prominent attention recently with a series of articles in the journal Cortex. A paper by Kenneth Paap of San Francisco State University and colleagues kicked off the fireworks in August, arguing that the evidence now suggests either no bilingual advantage exists, or it only occurs under certain as yet undetermined circumstances. Twenty-one commentaries and a summary by Paap and colleagues followed in October.


The authors were reacting to the intense optimism about the benefits of bilingualism generated by a large number of studies published over the last decade or so. Ellen Bialystok, a psychologist at York University, Toronto, and her colleagues, produced much of the important early work that helped to dismiss the outdated idea that bilingualism could be detrimental to children's intellectual development. Later studies went further, finding that bilingual children actually perform better than monolinguals in tests of “executive functions” –processes that control thought and behavior and enable complex cognitive tasks like problem solving .


A study published in 2004 made a particularly big splash. Bialystok and colleagues compared the cognitive control performance of elderly bilinguals and monolinguals. Not only was this the first study to find advantages in older bilingual adults, the results implied that being bilingual might mitigate age-related cognitive decline. Subsequent studies in patient populations have shown that being bilingual can delay the onset of dementia by about four to five years.


The measure Bialystok's group used, called the “Simon” task, involves asking participants to respond on their left or right, depending on the color of a box that appears on a screen. Researchers typically calculate how much longer participants take to react when the color they must respond to on their right appears on the left, or vice-versa, compared to when the location doesn't conflict. This test is designed to measure inhibitory control, since participants must suppress a reflex to respond with the hand on the same side as the box.


Executive function—another name for inhibitory control—consists of three components: inhibition, switching between tasks, and updating working memory (the capacity to hold information in mind in the face of distraction). A common explanation of the bilingual advantage suggests that the constant practice of suppressing the language they are not using exercises inhibition. Another possibility is that constantly switching between languages exerts executive function when switching among different tasks. These hypotheses inspired a massive research effort investigating the extent, mechanisms, and causes of the bilingual advantage, involving over 200 studies and a huge amount of data.


Paap and colleagues identified several problems with this body of evidence. When researchers study groups in natural settings outside the laboratory, they can't control factors that may differ between groups, such as socioeconomics, immigrant status, and cultural differences. Attempts to match these factors among groups or account for them statistically are inevitably imperfect, leaving the possibility that differences in performance are due something other than language skills. An even thornier problem has to do with causality. Does being bilingual influence cognition, or does a person's cognitive ability affect the probability of acquiring multiple languages?


The researchers also collected the results of all tests comparing executive functions between bilinguals and monolinguals published since 2011, finding that 83 percent of them found no difference between the two groups. There was also a tendency for studies with positive results to have used smaller samples, whereas those using larger study populations were more likely to find no effect. Smaller sample sizes have a greater probability of producing a spurious result by chance.


Here is where the debate heats up. Some “skeptics” accuse “believers” of dismissing dissenting views and ignoring calls for higher standards. Believers accuse skeptics of selectively citing studies that support their argument, while not taking account the large body of research that supports the bilingual advantage. Bialystok claims the majority of negative findings come from studies of young adults, in which benefits of bilingualism are not often found because most young adults are at peak performance regardless of language skills.


The impact of speaking more than one language, Bialystok says, is more often found in children and in older adults. “There is a huge amount of research the skeptics never cite showing that if you give executive control tasks to bilingual children, they perform better.” She also points to brain imaging studies that find differences even in the absence of performance differences—a claim that the skeptics take issue with. Bilingualism, the naysayers contend, could conceivably reorganize the brain without leading to any benefits.


There is also disagreement about results in older adults. Some studies show a delay of four to five years in the onset of Alzheimer's disease, but others have not found this difference. Psychologist Angela de Bruin, of the University of Edinburgh, says it could depend how you measure disease onset. Retrospective studies ask people with dementia to report when their symptoms began, but there have also been some research now that tracks people over time. “Studies tracking people as they grow older have often not found a difference,” de Bruin says.


All this bickering takes place against the backdrop of a “reproducibility crisis” across all of psychological science (and life sciences in general). Commentators point to a system that rewards researchers for publishing positive and novel results, rather than maintaining scientific rigor and resisting pressures that lead to selective reporting of results that inflate the number of spurious findings.


A key problem relates to publication bias—the tendency for more positive results to be published than negative ones because of a combination of researchers' decisions about what to submit and editors' decisions about what to publish. De Bruin and colleagues looked for evidence of such bias in bilingualism studies by examining 104 conference abstracts, from 1999 to 2012. They classified the abstracts as supportive of the benefits of bilingualism, challenging of the idea, or mixed results. They then looked for which studies were ultimately published.


The researchers found that 68 percent of positive studies were published, whereas only 29 percent of the negative ones were. “There's a massive gap, and the only way for us to progress is to have access to all the results,” de Bruin says. They also combined the data from published studies (using a technique called meta-analysis) to assess what the sum of evidence says. This produced a significant but small positive result, but one likely to be an over-estimate. “Even though the meta-analysis showed a small effect, we don't know how reliable it is, because it's based on a selective sample,” de Bruin says. (Specifically, the data yielded an effect size of 0.3—effect sizes are standardized measures normalized according to the variability of the data, with 0.3 indicating a “small” effect.)


Paap and colleagues argue the true effect could be zero, but stress this does not imply bilingualism has any negative consequences. There is no evidence of any disadvantage in speaking additional languages for executive function or intelligence, whereas benefits unrelated to cognition are numerous and profound. “We don't question whether it's better to speak more than one language," Paap says. "It makes travel easier and more rewarding, can open career doors and other opportunities, and enables one to speak to more people, develop a deeper understanding of another culture, and understand the world from another vantage point.”


Paap also acknowledges the possibility that the effect of bilingualism on cognition is real, but only occurs in specific circumstances. If only certain types of language experience produces an advantage, divergent results could result from studying different groups that varied in the age they learned a second language, amount of use and relative proficiency. But psychologist Claudia von Bastian, at University of Colorado, Boulder, and colleagues at the University of Zurich, recently published a study quantifying bilingualism along all these dimensions, using nine executive function measures, and found no effect of bilingualism on any aspect of executive function.


Evy Woumans of Ghent University, Belgium, thinks the advantages of a second language may relate to how often a person switches between languages. She and her colleagues recently published a study comparing balanced bilinguals who switch many times a day, with interpreters who switch infrequently. They found that better language switching proficiency was associated with better executive functioning, but only for balanced bilinguals, not interpreters. “This suggests control of languages [an ability to readily switch] leads to better executive functioning, not the other way around,” Woumans says. She also advocates moving beyond a yes or no debate, to investigate what determines whether advantages occur. “It's important for both camps to agree it's not straightforward—when you have that [acknowledgement] you can collaborate to try to find out why some people find something and others don't,” Woumans says. “Then we'll be a lot closer to finding answers.”


Wouman’s comment echoes calls from many skeptics for collaborations between proponents and critics of the bilingual advantage. These efforts would ideally involve ‘pre-registration” of studies, specifying the study design, measures used to assess study outcomes, and other analysis methods. Defining these study characteristics in advance would prevent the possibility of changing methodology mid-course, a practice that can inflate spurious findings. Cortex, in fact, announced an option for researchers to pre-register studies in 2013. Unfortunately, the fever pitch of this debate suggests the two sides are not likely to take advantage of this offer any time soon.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 12:15

Gonorrhea Could Soon Be Untreatable

Health and Medicine





Photo credit:

Gonorrhoeae bacteria in urethral exudate. Dr. Norman Jacobs/USCDC/public domain.



The United Kingdom's chief medical officer has sent a warning to all doctors that the sexually transmitted disease gonorrhea could evolve to the point where it cannot be treated, returning humanity to the pre-penicillin era. Such an outcome can be delayed, and possibly prevented entirely, if doctors prescribe drugs correctly, Dame Sally Davies claimed.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 11:42

Our Distance From Africa Predicts Our Number Of Harmful Genes

Plants and Animals





Photo credit:

A grass hut in the Kalahari Desert. Brenna M. Henn



Modern humans dispersed out of Africa and expanded into multiple continents around 50,000 years ago. Researchers studying the genetic impact of our species’ expansion reveal that the distance from sub-Saharan Africa can help predict the number of deleterious mutations in a genome. The findings are published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences this week. 

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 11:40

“Absurdly Inadequate”

CreationismHistory



Perhaps I begin to sound like a broken phonograph, but I find that chapter 28—“Scientists Condemn Evolution”—of William A. Williams’s The Evolution of Man Scientifically Disproved (1925) is the gift that keeps on giving. (The phrase, I find, was originally a marketing slogan for a phonograph, so there you go.) That chapter is chockablock with a lot of quotations from various scientific authorities. As it happens, I’ve already blogged here at the Science League of America about a number of them: including Lionel S. BealeAlbert Fleischmann (misspelled “Fleishman” by Williams), St. George Mivart (misspelled “Mivert” by Williams), Ernst HaeckelNathaniel S. Shaler (although Williams didn’t misattribute his words to Darwin), Oscar Fraas (misspelled “Traas” by Williams), and, most recently, Thomas Henry Huxley. And now it’s the turn of W. H. Thompson.



According to Williams, “Dr. W. H. Thompson, former president of NY Academy of Medicine, said: ‘The Darwinian theory is now rejected by the majority of biologists, as absurdly inadequate. It is absurd to rank man among the animals. His so called fellow animals, the primates—gorilla, orang and chimpanzee—can do nothing truly human.’” Where did Williams find the passage? Although he acknowledges taking his “testimonies” from earlier books, namely, Luther Townsend’s Collapse of Evolution (1905), Alfred W. McCann’s God—Or Gorilla (1922), and Philip Mauro’s Evolution at the Bar (1922), none of these contains the passage from Thompson. The earliest instance of the quotation I was able to find was in the August–September 1922 issue of Theological Monthly (“published by the Evangelical Lutheran Synod of Missouri, Ohio, and other states”), which was itself quoting from the May 9, 1922, issue of the Lutheran Church Herald.



As quoted in Theological Monthly, the passage appears thus: “Dr. William Hanna Thompson, president of the New York Academy of Medicine, says: ‘The Darwinian theory is now rejected by the majority of biologists as absurdly inadequate, owing to its principle’s being wholly negative. Selection of any kind does not produce anything.’” The minor divergences—whether Thompson’s initials or first and middle names are used; whether he is described as the president or the former president of the New York Academy of Medicine—aren’t important as compared to the major divergence. On the showing of Theological Monthly, Thompson is expressing opposition to Darwinism sensu the effectiveness of natural selection, as (for instance) Harold Begbie and D’Arcy Wentworth Thompson did, while according to Williams, Thompson is expressing opposition to Darwinism sensu the common ancestry of humans and primates.



Which is it? Well, the horse’s mouth appears to be a column, “The Mystery of Life and the Mystery of Man,” published in The New York Times for November 5, 1911, under the byline of Dr. William Hanna Thomson (above)—note the absence of a p from the surname. In a box by the headline, there appears:




The Darwinian theory is now rejected by the majority of biologists as absurdly inadequate, owing to its principle being wholly negative. Selection of any kind does not produce anything, but only chooses between that which already exists.



Evolution never was a cause, but a result, instead.



It is almost pathetic to read how Huxley and Darwin, in their day, fancied that because the primate homo [sic]—man—was so well in keeping with the evolution of the other primates, therefore they had scientifically accounted for man.



It is absurd to rank man among the animals. His so-called fellow-animals, the primates—gorilla, orang, and chimpanzee—can do nothing truly human.




Mirabile dictu, both Theological Monthly and Williams are apparently right: Thomson is evidently expressing opposition both to the effectiveness of natural selection in evolution (as in the first paragraph) and to the common ancestry of humans and primates (as in the third and fourth paragraphs).



But the situation isn’t quite so simple. The material in the box is excerpted from the column—perhaps by the editorial staff of the Times—and is therefore taken, to a degree, out of context. In particular, in the context of his column, Thomson is not so much objecting to the common ancestry of humans and primates—he refers without demur to “the appearance of the anthropoid apes, such as the gorilla, orang, and the chimpanzee, who together constitutes the zoological genius of the primates, which, in turn, culminated in the animal homo [sic]. Homo, or man, is as much an animal as any other animal”—as he is objecting to the idea that biology is capable of explaining what’s distinctive about humans. “The more this epoch is studied,” Thomson declaims, “the more impossible it is to account for the advent of the person [as opposed to the animal], man.” And what is it about humans that biology is incapable of explaining?



As it happens, Thomson thinks that humans are distinctive in possessing intellectual, creative, invisible, indivisible, and eternal personalities, unlike “the brutes that perish.” (The column is duly ornamented by a reproduction of Italo Sabatini’s “There Is No Death,” described as “a striking painting … showing the passing of the soul [apparently not invisible after all] from the body.” The Times reproduced the painting earlier in 1911, under the headline “Art and Spiritualism,” and then thriftily used it also to accompany Oliver Lodge’s 1913 article headlined “President of the British Association Also Believes that Discarnate Intelligence Under Certain Conditions May Interact with Us on the Material Side.”) Whatever there is to be said for or against the view, Thomson seems to regard it as consistent with human evolution, making Williams’s use of the passage illicit—which, by now, is hardly a surprise.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 10:30

Researchers Discover Glow-In-The-Dark Shark

Plants and Animals





Photo credit:

The newly discovered ninja lanternshark. Vásquez V.E., Dr. Douglas Long & Ross Robertson/JOSF



The fear created by the great white shark in "Jaws" has a lot to do with it being unseen for most of the movie. So spare a thought for the prey of this newly discovered shark, which has evolved to hunt unseen.


The ninja lanternshark has black skin that allows it to blend in with the depths of the ocean where it hunts. The black skin is not the only trick up the fin of this predator, as the shark also glows in the dark, a deadly combination.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 09:05

New Exploding Star Found On Christmas Day

Space





Photo credit:

The nova was found in the Triangulum Galaxy, shown. Slooh



In a timely occurrence, a new star appeared on Christmas day when a star exploded as a nova – a smaller version of a supernova. And the Slooh Observatory will be broadcasting live views of the nova and its parent galaxy at 2:30 p.m. EST (7:30 p.m. GMT) today.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 09:03

Marmosets Can Perceive Pitch Too

Plants and Animals





Photo credit:

Yunyan Wang/Johns Hopkins Medicine



Common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, are vocal little monkeys from South America with a hearing range similar to that of humans. And like us, it appears these New World primates can perceive pitch.


The findings, published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences this week, suggest that certain aspects of pitch perception emerged early in the evolution of primates. Our species have been separated by at least 40 million years. 

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 07:32

Second Transmissible Cancer Found In Tasmanian Devils

Plants and Animals





Photo credit:

Bernhard Richter/Shutterstock



Tasmanian devils, Sarcophilus harrisii, are being threatened with extinction by a transmissible cancer called devil facial tumor disease. This gruesome disease has been ravaging the little marsupial carnivores since it was first observed in 1996.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 07:26

Exoplanet Cores May Contain Compounds Impossible On Earth

Chemistry





Photo credit:

An artist's vision of Kepler 10-c, the discovery of which overthrew ideas of how large rocky planets could be. New modelling suggests such planets' interiors contain novel compounds. Harvard-Smithsonian Center for Astrophysics/David Aguilar



The intense pressures at the heart of rocky planets larger than Earth will cause elements to combine in ways they never do on Earth. Modeling suggests this could result in exceptionally powerful magnetic fields and much more rapid geologic activity at the planet's surface.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 07:26

Hybrid Electrolyte Fills Battery Gap

Chemistry





Photo credit:

Electric cars are only the most prominent of the many technologies that depend on advances in battery technology. otomobil/Shutterstock



In the quest to build batteries that will store electricity at prices we can afford, neither polymer nor ceramic electrolytes are proving satisfactory. A new solution combining features of both might bridge the gap to the clean energy future.

 •  0 comments  •  flag
Share on Twitter
Published on December 30, 2015 07:25

ريتشارد دوكنز's Blog

ريتشارد دوكنز
ريتشارد دوكنز isn't a Goodreads Author (yet), but they do have a blog, so here are some recent posts imported from their feed.
Follow ريتشارد دوكنز's blog with rss.